- Published: December 21, 2021
- Updated: December 21, 2021
- University / College: Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai
- Language: English
- Downloads: 14
Introduction
Interoception pertains to receiving, encoding, and representation of internal bodily signals in the brain, as well as their perception ( Cameron, 2001 ; Craig, 2002 ; Critchley et al., 2004 ). Through interoception we know when our heart is beating fast, when we need to take a deep breath, and when we are hungry, thirsty, hot, cold, nauseous, tired, or alert. It encompasses both the non-conscious bodily signals themselves and our conscious perception of them. Growing research has shown interoception not only to be crucial for homeostasis and allostasis (acute change to achieve homeostasis), but also central in a range of cognitive and emotional processes, including memory, decision-making, emotional processing, social interactions, and even consciousness, body ownership and a sense of self ( Critchley et al., 2001 ; Dunn et al., 2010 ; Shah et al., 2017 ; Berntson et al., 2018 ; Critchley and Garfinkel, 2018 ). By their nature, internal bodily processes are notoriously difficult to manipulate in experimental settings. The vagus nerve, the main cranial nerve in the human body known to be central in relaying visceral signals to the brain, is naturally implicated in interoception ( Critchley and Harrison, 2013 ; Quadt et al., 2019 ; Yoris et al., 2019 ). Yet surprisingly little research in this area has used vagus nerve stimulation (VNS) to modulate bodily signaling. So far, to the best of our knowledge, only one recent study has explicitly related VNS to interoception ( Villani et al., 2019 ). Here, we review the accumulated cognitive and clinical research on VNS and propose that this technique can indeed be used to modulate a wide range of interoception-related processes.
The vagus nerve (cranial nerve X) is the longest and one of the most widely distributed nerves in the body ( vagus in Latin means “ wandering”). As part of the parasympathetic division of the autonomic nervous system, the vagus nerve conveys information between visceral organs (i. e., organs located in the thoracic and abdominal cavities, including the heart) and the brain ( Berthoud and Neuhuber, 2000 ; Craig, 2002 ; Critchley and Harrison, 2013 ). About 80% of the vagus nerve is composed of afferent fibers, projecting to the nucleus tractus solitarii (NTS) in the medulla, before being relayed further to other brainstem nuclei (broadly implicated in homeostatic control) and higher-order structures, including the thalamus, hippocampus, amygdala, and insula ( Goehler et al., 2000 ; Saper, 2002 ). VNS involves electrical stimulation of the afferent fibers of the vagus nerve, either in an invasive variant (where the stimulator is implanted in the patient’s body) or a non-invasive variant where the stimulation is delivered transcutaneously through the auricular branch of the vagus nerve (tVNS, aVNS, or taVNS). Both VNS and tVNS reliably induce activation in the same brain areas ( Narayanan et al., 2002 ; Frangos et al., 2015 ; Badran et al., 2018a ). These areas, and their projections, and especially the insula, have been shown to be implicated in interoception ( Craig, 2002 ).
Interoception has been conceptually divided into hierarchical levels ( Garfinkel et al., 2015 ; Quadt et al., 2018 ), from low-level visceral afferents and their preconscious effects on cognitive or affective processes to psychological dimensions and metacognitive awareness of internal bodily processes. Because the vagus nerve is the main pathway relaying visceral signals into the brain, we propose that (t)VNS could affect interoception at each of those levels. Recent years have also seen a growing interest in the role of interoception in mental illness, proposing that interoceptive dysfunction can contribute to impairments in social, cognitive, behavioral, affective, and somatic processes associated with certain psychopathologies such as anxiety, depression, eating disorders, addiction, or post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) ( Paulus and Stein, 2010 ; Harshaw, 2015 ; Khalsa et al., 2018 ). Those dysfunctions can occur at any level of interoceptive processing. Proposed mechanisms include aberrant processing of the afferent signal (e. g., noisy inputs) or weight given to its representations (e. g., overweighted interoceptive signal affecting subsequent evaluation of affective stimuli), abnormal expectations about bodily states, attentional or cognitive biases (e. g., hypervigilance to bodily states), or psychological biases (e. g., weak insight into one’s bodily state and its relevance to environmental context) ( Khalsa et al., 2018 ; Petzschner et al., 2017 ; Gu et al., 2019 ).
This paper provides an overview of the research on the effects of VNS on interoception-related processes (both cognitive and clinical), from the lowest to highest level, and relates those findings to embodied models of brain function.
Low-Level Afferent Interoceptive Signal
The low-level visceral signal, such as baroreceptor activity or blood oxygenation, is not consciously perceived unless there is a problem. However, according to embodied, interoception-based accounts of the conscious self, access to and evaluation of the signal from the physical body is necessary for the conscious “ self” to arise in the first place ( Seth et al., 2012 ; Seth, 2013 ; Cleeremans et al., 2020 ). Stimulated NTS, the projection area of the vagus nerve, activates the dorsal raphe and other areas known to control alertness ( George et al., 2000 ). For this reason, VNS was proposed as a fruitful therapy for disorders of sleep or even consciousness ( George et al., 2000 ; Naritoku et al., 2003 ), and was subsequently shown to promote improved sleep in rats ( Rong et al., 2019 ) and human adults ( Bretherton et al., 2019 ). Impressively Corazzol et al. (2017), in a single case study, demonstrated that long-term VNS may improve patient condition even after years in persistent unresponsive wakefulness (vegetative state), warranting future research in this area. This finding corroborates the proposal that a sense of conscious self is embodied – the experience of owning and identifying with one’s own body, and the experience of first-person perspective, are directly associated with multi-level representations of physiological condition of the body ( Seth et al., 2012 ; Seth, 2013 ; Cleeremans et al., 2020 ). As such, (t)VNS may play a pivotal role in helping to restore this conscious, embodied sensation. This hypothesis could be further reinforced if classical paradigms exploring the sense of self, such as the rubber-hand or full-body illusion or sense of agency, proved to be modulated by (t)VNS.
In the cognitive literature, a demonstration that VNS directly manipulates the read-outs of basic visceral afferent signals has yet to be given. The most promising candidate is the modulation of Heartbeat Evoked Potential (HEP) amplitude. The cardiac signal is one of the most widely explored low-level afferent signals, reflected in electroencephalography as an event-related potential synchronized with cardiac R-peaks. HEP has been shown to predict individual heartbeat perception ( Pollatos et al., 2005 ; Terhaar et al., 2012 ), and was proposed to reflect interoceptive belief updating ( Ainley et al., 2016 ) and affective predictions ( Gentsch et al., 2018 ; Marshall et al., 2018 ). It has also been shown to be modulated by internal (vs. external) attentional focus, proposed to reflect interoceptive precision ( Petzschner et al., 2019 ). The HEP has been shown to originate in the insula, the brain region assumed to be key for interoception ( Park et al., 2017 ), and activated through (t)VNS (e. g., Badran et al., 2018a ). Interestingly, Park et al. (2017) confirmed the HEP’s functional role in self-consciousness by demonstrating HEP modulations as a response to an experimentally-induced altered sense of self-identification (full-body illusion). This result supports the proposal that interoceptive information is a crucial substrate of the sense of self and body ownership ( Sierra and David, 2011 ; Critchley and Harrison, 2013 ; Crucianelli et al., 2018 ). Additionally, the sense of body ownership has been shown to be impaired after insula lesions ( Karnath, 2005 ; Gandola et al., 2012 ; Moro et al., 2016 ). Given that (t)VNS reliably activates the interoceptive network (including the insula), it could constitute a tool for manipulating low-level interoceptive signals and their read-outs, such as the HEP, for research into consciousness, sense of self and body ownership.
Preconscious Impacts on Cognitive Processes
While the previous, lowest level of the interoceptive hierarchy was concerned with the mere communication and detection of bodily afferents, the second, preconscious level refers to the early stages of central processing and pertains to the impact the afferent visceral signals have on cognitive and affective processing. The cardiac signal in particular has been shown to play a part in emotion processing ( Bechara and Naqvi, 2004 ). There is evidence that processing of emotional faces is enhanced at systole compared to diastole ( Garfinkel and Critchley, 2016 ), and the learning of fearful face-name pairs is better at systole than at diastole (interestingly, just in people with heightened interoceptive ability, and not for happy or neutral faces; Pfeifer et al., 2017 ). Higher interoceptive ability (encompassing perception and confidence in one’s perception of bodily signals; see next section) correlates with recognition memory for emotional pictures ( Pollatos and Schandry, 2008 ) and words ( Werner et al., 2010 ). “ Gut feeling” is commonly used to mean intuition, pointing to a popular sensation of visceral signals influencing decisions. Adaptive behavior and optimal decision-making have indeed been proposed to be supported, or even guided, by visceral signals, where the physiological state of the body provides a reference frame for homeostatic or motivational value of given choice options ( Damasio, 1994 ; Bechara, 2004 ; Gu and FitzGerald, 2014 ; Maniscalco and Rinaman, 2018 ). Although the (t)VNS literature has not focused on interoception, studies have shown that this technique modulates similar interoceptive processes to those mentioned above. Emerging evidence suggests that tVNS can improve facial emotion recognition ( Colzato et al., 2017a ), though, interestingly, not from entire bodies ( Sellaro et al., 2018 ). Elsewhere, preliminary experimental evidence suggests that VNS can improve decision-making on the Iowa Gambling Task ( Martin et al., 2004 ).
Another example of a physical state influencing cognitive function is the phenomenon of memory enhancement during somatosensory arousal. Traumatic memories are remembered particularly vividly. This phenomenon is crucially reliant on vagal transmission of various neuromodulators ( Flood and Morley, 1988 ; Williams and Jensen, 1993 ; Nogueira et al., 1994 ; Talley et al., 2002 ). VNS may induce a state similar to arousal, most likely linked to secretion of noradrenaline and acetylcholine in the brain, neurotransmitters known to mediate attention ( Martino et al., 2007 ; Klinkenberg et al., 2011 ). Studies have shown that (t)VNS may improve declarative memory retention ( Clark et al., 1999 ; Ghacibeh et al., 2006 ; Jacobs et al., 2015 ; Broncel et al., 2020 ; Giraudier et al., 2020 ), even in patients with Alzheimer’s Disease ( Clark et al., 1999 ; Sjogren et al., 2002 ; Merrill et al., 2006 ). Other authors have found mixed results in single-session studies (see Vonck et al., 2014 for review), depending on the stimulation settings (0. 5 mA being optimal). Clinical interventions have capitalized on this memory-enhancing effect of (t)VNS to strengthen the formation of adaptive memories and behaviors after brain damage, e. g., “ targeted plasticity” interventions ( Hays et al., 2013 ). Interestingly, there is mixed evidence whether long-term VNS may lead to general memory improvement, which was not observed in those undergoing chronic VNS treatment for epilepsy, but was in depression ( Aaronson et al., 2013 ; Vonck et al., 2014 ). Vonck et al. (2014) point out that cognitive dysfunctions are inherent to clinical depression and known to improve with improvement of depressive symptoms. This ties in with accounts of depression linking it to malfunctioning interoceptive evaluation of bodily signals ( Barrett and Simmons, 2015 ; Quadt et al., 2018 ). Chronic aberrant interoceptive processing has maladaptive allostatic consequences for dealing with stress and, in particular, inflammation, which frequently cooccurs with depression. Indeed ( Howland, 2014 ) points that the relationship between depression and inflammation may be mediated by the vagus nerve.
Malfunctioning interoceptive evaluation of bodily signals is also implicated in anxiety and panic. It may result from distorted interoceptive learning, when a benign sensation is experienced in the context of an initial panic attack, resulting in rapid conditioning. Overriding such strongly conditioned responses remains a challenge for most therapies. Fear extinction, a removal of conditioned response, is a gold standard therapy for PTSD ( Genheimer et al., 2017 ), yet for many it remains not entirely effective, calling for its enhancement, potentially with VNS or tVNS. So far, however, the results are mixed, with promising results of VNS on fear extinction in rats ( Peña et al., 2013 , 2014 ; Noble et al., 2017 ; Souza et al., 2019 , 2020 ), and with varied success of tVNS in humans ( Burger et al., 2016 , 2017 ; Genheimer et al., 2017 ; Szeska et al., 2020 ), which may depend on particular stimulation parameters ( Hansen, 2019 ).
Psychological Dimensions: Interoceptive Accuracy, Sensibility, and Awareness
Psychological dimensions of interception refer to the conscious perception of bodily signals ( Garfinkel et al., 2015 ). These dimensions have been conceptualized across three levels: interoceptive accuracy (referring to objective accuracy in perceiving a bodily signal, e. g., one’s own heartbeat), sensibility (subjective beliefs about the ability to perceive own bodily sensations), and awareness (the correspondence between accuracy and confidence, i. e., a metacognitive aspect of interoceptive ability). Interoceptive accuracy and awareness are typically quantified with performance on bodily signal perception tasks, such as heartbeat counting or detection tasks (e. g., Schandry, 1981 ). Those who perform well on heartbeat detection tasks also tend to experience greater arousal and higher HEP amplitudes for emotional pictures ( Herbert et al., 2007 ). Performance on such tasks is shown to correlate with the intensity of one’s own emotions ( Wiens et al., 2000 ), perception of others’ emotions ( Terasawa et al., 2014 ), and decision-making ( Werner et al., 2009 ; Kandasamy et al., 2016 ), although the causal relations are still unclear.
The role of tVNS on the psychological dimensions of interoception was directly tested by Villani et al. (2019). They found tVNS to improve participants’ ability to correctly identify (but not count) their own heartbeats (interoceptive accuracy). Furthermore, participants reported higher confidence in their decisions under tVNS, but this did not lead to enhanced interoceptive awareness. This promising result, in light of the range of phenomena shown to be improved in individuals with higher interoceptive accuracy outlined above, offers an avenue for further research.
Deficits in interoceptive processing at this level are related to poor processing or evaluation of own bodily signals (e. g., impaired insight into one’s internal state and its relevance to environmental context; e. g., Paulus and Stein, 2010 ; Petzschner et al., 2017 ), and can predict emotional and affective psychopathology. Individuals with alexithymia, an impairment in recognizing one’s emotions which often cooccurs with autism (ASC), show reduced interoceptive accuracy ( Ernst et al., 2013 ), as well as reduced brain activation in the insular cortex ( Bird et al., 2010 ). Evidence for a reduction in interoceptive accuracy in the autistic population is mixed ( Quadt et al., 2018 ), and correlated with co-existing alexithymia ( Shah et al., 2016 ). Interestingly, autistic individuals tend to have elevated confidence (sensibility) relative to their performance accuracy ( Garfinkel et al., 2016 ). A similar discrepancy, termed “ interoceptive trait prediction error,” has been found in anxiety disorders ( Paulus and Stein, 2006 ). Direct interventions with tVNS have been proposed for autism ( Engineer et al., 2017 ) and anxiety ( George et al., 2008 ). tVNS has been shown to alleviate some symptoms of ASC in epileptic patients with this comorbidity (see Levy et al., 2010 ; Jin and Kong, 2017 for review), which we hypothesize to be modulated by improvement in their interpretative processing. More research is needed to correlate treatment-induced change in ASC symptomatology and interoceptive accuracy, sensibility, and awareness.
Aberrant performance on interoceptive tasks (usually lower accuracy) has been shown in eating disorders (ED), such as anorexia nervosa ( Pollatos et al., 2008 ; Van den Bergh et al., 2017 ), along with impaired ability to differentiate hunger and satiety, and reduced response to emotional states ( Fassino et al., 2004 ). Together these characteristics suggest weakened interoceptive processing. Vagus nerve stimulation is slowly being recognized as a potential treatment to regulate food craving ( Wernicke et al., 1993 ; Boveja and Widhany, 2003 ) proposed that vagal signal suppression could be helpful in treating obesity, signal stimulation in anorexia, and intermittent stimulation in bulimia, though experimental research is still needed. Further work might also identify whether the regulation of food cravings leads to changes in performance on interoceptive tasks.
Finally, pain has also been described in terms of interoceptive processing ( Craig, 2003 ; Khalsa et al., 2009 ), with the vagus nerve crucial in relaying somatosensory sensations. Pain is usually caused by the activation of nociceptors and nociceptive pathways ( Meyer et al., 2006 ), but it is also known to occur without corresponding activity, and nociceptors can be active without the sensation of pain (e. g., lack of reported pain by soldiers during battle, despite severe injuries), and be modulated by psychological state ( Melzack et al., 1982 ; Mariana von Mohr, 2019 ). Deficits in interoceptive accuracy have been reported in patients with fibromyalgia ( Duschek et al., 2015 ), lower back pain ( Mehling et al., 2013 ) and migraines (see ( Lernia et al., 2016 ) for review). VNS has been shown to modulate the sensation of pain in fibromyalgia ( Lange et al., 2011 ) and migraines ( Barbanti et al., 2015 ). Pain perception has been also reported to be reduced in patients treated with VNS for depression ( Borckardt et al., 2005 ). This points toward the conclusion that vagus nerve stimulation affects interoceptive processing of pain, though more research is needed to elucidate the causes of individual differences in response to treatment.
Metacognitive Level
The final, metacognitive level represents an executive dimension. It refers to one’s ability to flexibly switch between attending to and utilizing interoceptive and exteroceptive information in an adaptive manner. Though direct tests on such tasks are yet to be done, there is promising evidence that tVNS facilitates attentional switching in conflict situations, such as a number version of the Simon task ( Fischer et al., 2018 ), allows rapid attentional adaptation ( Colzato et al., 2017b ), and improves response selection in sequential action ( Jongkees et al., 2018 ). In the clinical domain, tVNS has been shown to reduce temper outbursts in Prader-Willi Syndrome ( Manning et al., 2019 ), further reinforcing the notion that it improves interoceptive processing at the high level implicated in executive control.
A VNS influence on metacognition may also account for the rare adverse side effect observed in 4 epilepsy patients, namely hallucinations and psychosis ( De Herdt et al., 2003 ), most likely caused by acutely increased alertness and decreased sedation through VNS. Patients with mild or severe intellectual disability may be specifically prone to the development of these symptoms. We suggest that in cases where cognitive resources for the appraisal of peripheral, somatosensory information are impaired, augmenting the signal strength can lead to systemic overload and malfunction.
Discussion
The range of processes which can be affected by stimulating the vagus nerve points to the sheer scale of visceral influence. The presented literature can be unified under the theoretical frameworks which posit that affective and cognitive states are continuously interpreted through, and biased by, the body’s internal states. While the primary function of interoceptive signals is to feed the brain a continuous stream of information on the internal state of the organism so as to ensure survival, there is increasing consensus that they also fundamentally inform motivational states, adaptive behavior and emotion ( Damasio, 2010 ; Critchley and Harrison, 2013 ; Seth, 2013 ; Barrett and Simmons, 2015 ; Critchley and Garfinkel, 2018 ; Seth and Tsakiris, 2018 ). According to this view, the brain interprets its current environmental challenges in light of the concurrent state of the body. The evidence of VNS influence on higher-order processes can thus lend support to models of brain function that assume a causal visceral dimension.
The successful application of VNS in clinical settings, briefly outlined here in the context of interoception, also corroborates the idea that a number of clinical conditions may have an interoceptive dimension. The visceral contribution has been noted for anxiety, depression, and PTSD, and many of the conditions discussed above have overlapping symptoms ( Khalsa et al., 2018 ). For example, ( Howland, 2014 ) pointed out that the relationship between depression, inflammation, metabolic syndrome, and heart disease may be mediated by the vagus nerve. VNS research can lead to greater understanding of the interoceptive dimension in clinical conditions, giving rise to future treatments.
While VNS has been enjoying increasing clinical success and is a promising tool for interoceptive manipulation, it is noteworthy that the optimal experimental protocols are still a work in progress, and should be carefully considered on an individual basis. Considerations under ongoing debate include optimal stimulation locations on the ear ( Burger and Verkuil, 2018 ), potential differences in signal paths between VNS and tVNS, and the longevity of the effect after stimulation ceases. It seems that although cognitive effects of VNS may be detectable after short periods of stimulation, even 20 min (e. g., Colzato et al., 2017b ), attenuation of certain clinical symptoms may require much longer stimulation durations (e. g., Manning et al., 2019 ) point to reduction in number and severity of temper outbursts in Prader-Willi Syndrome with 4 h/daily stimulation (as recommended for epilepsy), applied for 6–9 months, but a prompt return of the symptoms in all 5 participants when the stimulation was subsequently reduced to 2 h. Researchers should also consider making age related adjustments, e. g., ( Koo et al., 2001 ) point out that in children younger than 12 stimulation settings may require a higher stimulus current or longer pulse along with lower stimulus frequency than adults (e. g., stimulation at 20 Hz or lower instead of e. g., 30 Hz), as a child’s vagus nerve has slower conduction velocity. The effects of VNS on heart rate need to be considered as well.(Badran et al., 2018b) have shown that certain settings (pulse width 500 μs and frequency 10 Hz) are likely to lower the heart rate.
As for limitations of using VNS to modulate interoception, VNS cannot afford selective targeting of aspects of the interoceptive signal which may be of specific interest (e. g., heart, stomach, breath). As such, protocols should be designed to capitalize on VNS’s capacity to manipulate the strength of the afferent signal. Functional neuroimaging studies could be particularly amenable to this. For a thorough treatment of the limitations, see Kaniusas et al. (2019).
To conclude, vagus nerve stimulation affords a novel experimental approach to studying interoception and its role in cognitive and emotional processes and disorders at increasing levels of complexity. With developments in the method and optimal protocols gaining momentum, vagus nerve stimulation may prove a fruitful new gateway to interoception.
Author Contributions
All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.
Funding
AP gratefully acknowledged the funding from the National Science Centre in Poland (Grant No. 2016/21/D/HS2/02493) as well as the contribution from the Faculty of Psychology, University of Warsaw. LS gratefully acknowledged the funding from the Dr. Mortimer and Theresa Sackler Foundation, which funds the Sackler Centre for Consciousness Science, as well as funding from School of Psychology at the University of Sussex.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
We thank the community of implicit learning researchers, especially the organizers of the 8th Implicit Learning Seminar in Tromso in 2019 (Elisabeth Norman & Axel Cleeremans). We met and began discussing these ideas there.
References
Aaronson, S. T., Carpenter, L. L., Conway, C. R., Reimherr, F. W., Lisanby, S. H., Schwartz, T. L., et al. (2013). Vagus nerve stimulation therapy randomized to different amounts of electrical charge for treatment-resistant depression: acute and chronic effects. Brain Stimul. 6, 631–640. doi: 10. 1016/j. brs. 2012. 09. 013
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Ainley, V., Apps, M. A. J., Fotopoulou, A., and Tsakiris, M. (2016). ‘ Bodily precision’: a predictive coding account of individual differences in interoceptive accuracy. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 371: 20160003. doi: 10. 1098/rstb. 2016. 0003
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Badran, B. W., Dowdle, L. T., Mithoefer, O. J., LaBate, N. T., Coatsworth, J., Brown, J. C., et al. (2018a). Neurophysiologic effects of transcutaneous auricular vagus nerve stimulation (taVNS) via electrical stimulation of the tragus: a concurrent taVNS/fMRI study and review. Brain Stimul. 11, 492–500. doi: 10. 1016/j. brs. 2017. 12. 009
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Badran, B. W., Mithoefer, O. J., Summer, C. E., LaBate, N. T., Glusman, C. E., Badran, A. W., et al. (2018b). Short trains of transcutaneous auricular vagus nerve stimulation (taVNS) have parameter-specific effects on heart rate. Brain Stimul. 11, 699–708. doi: 10. 1016/j. brs. 2018. 04. 004
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Barbanti, P., Grazzi, L., Egeo, G., Padovan, A. M., Liebler, E., and Bussone, G. (2015). Non-invasive vagus nerve stimulation for acute treatment of high-frequency and chronic migraine: an open-label study. J. Headache Pain 16, 61. doi: 10. 1186/s10194-015-0542-4
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F., and Simmons, W. K. (2015). Interoceptive predictions in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 419–429. doi: 10. 1038/nrn3950
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Bechara, A. (2004). The role of emotion in decision-making: Evidence from neurological patients with orbitofrontal damage. Brain Cogn. 55, 30–40. doi: 10. 1016/j. bandc. 2003. 04. 001
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Bechara, A., and Naqvi, N. (2004). Listening to your heart: interoceptive awareness as a gateway to feeling. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 102–103. doi: 10. 1038/nn0204-102
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Berntson, G., Gianaros, P. J., and Tsakiris, M. (2018). “ Interoception and the autonomic nervous system: bottom-up meets top-down,” in Interoceptive Mind: From Homeostasis to Awareness , eds M. Tsakiris and H. De Preester (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 3–25.
Berthoud, H.-R., and Neuhuber, W. L. (2000). Functional and chemical anatomy of the afferent vagal system. Auton. Neurosci. 85, 1–17. doi: 10. 1016/s1566-0702(00)00215-0
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Bird, G., Silani, G., Brindley, R., White, S., Frith, U., and Singer, T. (2010). Empathic brain responses in insula are modulated by levels of alexithymia but not autism. Brain 133, 1515–1525. doi: 10. 1093/brain/awq060
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Borckardt, J. J., Kozel, F. A., Anderson, B., Walker, A., and George, M. S. (2005). Vagus nerve stimulation affects pain perception in depressed adults. Pain Res. Manag. 10, 9–14. doi: 10. 1155/2005/256472
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Boveja, B. R., and Widhany, A. (2003). Method and System for Providing Therapy for bulimia/Eating Disorders by Providing Electrical Pulses to Vagus nerve(s). Washington, DC: U. S. Patent and Trademark Office. U. S. Patent No US 2005/0192644 A1. Available online at: https://patents. google. com/patent/US20050192644
Bretherton, B., Atkinson, L., Murray, A., Clancy, J., Deuchars, S., and Deuchars, J. (2019). Effects of transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation in individuals aged 55 years or above: potential benefits of daily stimulation. Aging 11, 4836–4857. doi: 10. 18632/aging. 102074
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Broncel, A., Bocian, R., Kłos-Wojtczak, P., Kulbat-Warycha, K., and Konopacki, J. (2020). Vagal nerve stimulation as a promising tool in the improvement of cognitive disorders. Brain Res. Bull. 155, 37–47. doi: 10. 1016/j. brainresbull. 2019. 11. 011
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Burger, A. M., and Verkuil, B. (2018). Transcutaneous nerve stimulation via the tragus: are we really stimulating the vagus nerve? Brain Stimula. 11, 945–946. doi: 10. 1016/j. brs. 2018. 03. 018
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Burger, A. M., Verkuil, B., Diest, I. V., der Does, W. V., Thayer, J. F., and Brosschot, J. F. (2016). The effects of transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation on conditioned fear extinction in humans. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 132, 49–56. doi: 10. 1016/j. nlm. 2016. 05. 007
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Burger, A. M., Verkuil, B., Fenlon, H., Thijs, L., Cools, L., Miller, H. C., et al. (2017). Mixed evidence for the potential of non-invasive transcutaneous vagal nerve stimulation to improve the extinction and retention of fear. Behav. Res. Ther. 97, 64–74. doi: 10. 1016/j. brat. 2017. 07. 005
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Cameron, O. G. (2001). Interoception: the inside StoryA model for psychosomatic processes. Psychosom. Med. 63, 697–710. doi: 10. 1097/00006842-200109000-00001
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Clark, K. B., Naritoku, D. K., Smith, D. C., Browning, R. A., and Jensen, R. A. (1999). Enhanced recognition memory following vagus nerve stimulation in human subjects. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 94–98. doi: 10. 1038/4600
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Cleeremans, A., Achoui, D., Beauny, A., Keuninckx, L., Martin, J.-R., Muñoz-Moldes, S., et al. (2020). Learning to be conscious. Trends Cogn. Sci. 24, 112–123. doi: 10. 1016/j. tics. 2019. 11. 011
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Colzato, L. S., Sellaro, R., and Beste, C. (2017a). Darwin revisited: the vagus nerve is a causal element in controlling recognition of others emotions. Cortex 92, 95–102. doi: 10. 1016/j. cortex. 2017. 03. 017
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Colzato, L. S., Wolters, G., and Peifer, C. (2017b). Transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation (tVNS) modulates flow experience. Exp. Brain Res. 236, 253–257. doi: 10. 1007/s00221-017-5123-0
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Corazzol, M., Lio, G., Lefevre, A., Deiana, G., Tell, L., André-Obadia, N., et al. (2017). Restoring consciousness with vagus nerve stimulation. Curr. Biol. 27, R994–R996. doi: 10. 1016/j. cub. 2017. 07. 060
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Craig, A. D. B. (2003). Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Curr. Opin. iNeurobiol. 13, 500–505. doi: 10. 1016/s0959-4388(03)00090-4
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Craig, B. (2002). How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 655–666. doi: 10. 1038/nrn894
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Critchley, H. D., and Garfinkel, S. N. (2018). The influence of physiological signals on cognition. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 19, 13–18. doi: 10. 1016/j. cobeha. 2017. 08. 014
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Critchley, H. D., and Harrison, N. A. (2013). Visceral Influences on Brain and Behavior. Neuron 77, 624–638. doi: 10. 1016/j. neuron. 2013. 02. 008
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Critchley, H. D., Mathias, C. J., and Dolan, R. J. (2001). Neuroanatomical basis for first- and second-order representations of bodily states. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 207–212. doi: 10. 1038/84048
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Critchley, H. D., Wiens, S., Rotshtein, P., Öhman, A., and Dolan, R. J. (2004). Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 189–195. doi: 10. 1038/nn1176
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Crucianelli, L., Krahe, C., Jenkinson, P. M., and Fotopoulou, A. K. (2018). Interoceptive ingredients of body ownership: affective touch and cardiac awareness in the rubber hand illusion. Cortex 104, 180–192. doi: 10. 1016/j. cortex. 2017. 04. 018
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Damasio, A. (1994). Descartes’ Error. New York, NY: Vintage.
Damasio, A. (2010). Self Comes to Mind: Constructing the Conscious Self. New York, NY: Random House.
De Herdt, V., Boon, P., Vonck, K., Goossens, L., Nieuwenhuis, L., Paemeleire, K., et al. (2003). Are psychotic symptoms related to vagus nerve stimulation in epilepsy patients? Acta Neurol. Belg. 103, 170–175.
Dunn, B. D., Galton, H. C., Morgan, R., Evans, D., Oliver, C., Meyer, M., et al. (2010). Listening to your heart. Psychol. Sci. 21, 1835–1844. doi: 10. 1177/0956797610389191
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Duschek, S., Montoro, C. I., and del Paso, G. A. R. (2015). Diminished interoceptive awareness in fibromyalgia syndrome. Behav. Med. 43, 100–107. doi: 10. 1080/08964289. 2015. 1094442
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Engineer, C. T., Hays, S. A., and Kilgard, M. P. (2017). Vagus nerve stimulation as a potential adjuvant to behavioral therapy for autism and other neurodevelopmental disorders. J. Neurodev. Disord. 9: 20. doi: 10. 1186/s11689-017-9203-z
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Ernst, J., Böker, H., Hättenschwiler, J., Schüpbach, D., Northoff, G., Seifritz, E., et al. (2013). The association of interoceptive awareness and alexithymia with neurotransmitter concentrations in insula and anterior cingulate. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 857–863. doi: 10. 1093/scan/nst058
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Fassino, S., Pierò, A., Gramaglia, C., and Abbate-Daga, G. (2004). Clinical psychopathological and personality correlates of interoceptive awareness in anorexia nervosa. Bulimia nervosa and Obesity. Psychopathology 37, 168–174. doi: 10. 1159/000079420
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Fischer, R., Ventura-Bort, C., Hamm, A., and Weymar, M. (2018). Transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation (tVNS) enhances conflict-triggered adjustment of cognitive control. Cogn. Affect. Behavi. Neurosci. 18, 680–693. doi: 10. 3758/s13415-018-0596-2
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Flood, J. F., and Morley, J. E. (1988). Effects of bombesin and gastrin-releasing peptide on memory processing. Brain Res. 460, 314–322. doi: 10. 1016/0006-8993(88)90375-7
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Frangos, E., Ellrich, J., and Komisaruk, B. R. (2015). Non-invasive access to the vagus nerve central projections via electrical stimulation of the external Ear: fMRI Evidence in humans. Brain Stimul. 8, 624–636. doi: 10. 1016/j. brs. 2014. 11. 018
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Gandola, M., Invernizzi, P., Sedda, A., Ferrè, E. R., Sterzi, R., Sberna, M., et al. (2012). An anatomical account of somatoparaphrenia. Cortex 48, 1165–1178. doi: 10. 1016/j. cortex. 2011. 06. 012
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Garfinkel, S. N., and Critchley, H. D. (2016). Threat and the body: how the heart supports fear processing. Trends Cogn. Sci. 20, 34–46. doi: 10. 1016/j. tics. 2015. 10. 005
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Garfinkel, S. N., Seth, A. K., Barrett, A. B., Suzuki, K., and Critchley, H. D. (2015). Knowing your own heart: distinguishing interoceptive accuracy from interoceptive awareness. Biolo. Psychol. 104, 65–74. doi: 10. 1016/j. biopsycho. 2014. 11. 004
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Garfinkel, S. N., Tiley, C., OKeeffe, S., Harrison, N. A., Seth, A. K., and Critchley, H. D. (2016). Discrepancies between dimensions of interoception in autism: implications for emotion and anxiety. Biol. Psychol. 114, 117–126. doi: 10. 1016/j. biopsycho. 2015. 12. 003
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Genheimer, H., Andreatta, M., Asan, E., and Pauli, P. (2017). Reinstatement of contextual conditioned anxiety in virtual reality and the effects of transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation in humans. Scie. Rep. 7: 17886. doi: 10. 1038/s41598-017-18183-3
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Gentsch, A., Sel, A., Marshall, A. C., and Schütz-Bosbach, S. (2018). Affective interoceptive inference: evidence from heart-beat evoked brain potentials. Hum. Brain Mapp. 40, 20–33. doi: 10. 1002/hbm. 24352
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
George, M. S., Sackeim, H. A., Rush, A. J., Marangell, L. B., Nahas, Z., Husain, M. M., et al. (2000). Vagus nerve stimulation: a new tool for brain research and therapy. Biol. Psychiatry 47, 287–295. doi: 10. 1016/s0006-3223(99)00308-x
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
George, M. S., Ward, H. E., Ninan, P. T., Pollack, M., Nahas, Z., Anderson, B., et al. (2008). A pilot study of vagus nerve stimulation (VNS) for treatment-resistant anxiety disorders. Brain Stimul. 1, 112–121. doi: 10. 1016/j. brs. 2008. 02. 001
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Ghacibeh, G. A., Shenker, J. I., Shenal, B., Uthman, B. M., and Heilman, K. M. (2006). Effect of vagus nerve stimulation on creativity and cognitive flexibility. Epilepsy Behav. 8, 720–725. doi: 10. 1016/j. yebeh. 2006. 03. 008
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Giraudier, M., Ventura-Bort, C., and Weymar, M. (2020). Transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation improves high confidence recognition memory but not emotional word processing. Front. Psychol. 11: 1276. doi: 10. 3389/fpsyg. 2020. 01276
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Goehler, L. E., Gaykema, R. P. A., Hansen, M. K., Anderson, K., Maier, S. F., and Watkins, L. R. (2000). Vagal immune-to-brain communication: a visceral chemosensory pathway. Auton. Neurosci. 85, 49–59. doi: 10. 1016/s1566-0702(00)00219-8
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Gu, X., and FitzGerald, T. H. B. (2014). Interoceptive inference: homeostasis and decision making. Trends Cogni. Sci. 18, 269–270. doi: 10. 1016/j. tics. 2014. 02. 001
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Gu, X., FitzGerald, T. H. B., and Friston, K. (2019). Modelling subjective belief states in computational psychiatry: interoceptive inference as candidate framework. Psychopharmacology 236, 2405–2412. doi: 10. 1007/s00213-019-05300-5
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Hansen, N. (2019). Memory reinforcement and attenuation by activating the human locus coeruleus via transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation. Front. Neurosci. 12: 955. doi: 10. 3389/fnins. 2018. 00955
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Harshaw, C. (2015). Interoceptive dysfunction: toward an integrated framework for understanding somatic and affective disturbance in depression. Psychol. Bull. 141, 311–363. doi: 10. 1037/a0038101
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Hays, S. A., Rennaker, R. L., and Kilgard, M. P. (2013). “ Targeting Plasticity with vagus nerve stimulation to treat neurological disease,” in Changing Brains – Applying Brain Plasticity to Advance and Recover Human Ability , eds M. Nahum, M. Merzenich, and T. van Vleet (Amsterdam: Elsevier), 275–299. doi: 10. 1016/b978-0-444-63327-9. 00010-2
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Herbert, B. M., Pollatos, O., and Schandry, R. (2007). Interoceptive sensitivity and emotion processing: an EEG study. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 65, 214–227. doi: 10. 1016/j. ijpsycho. 2007. 04. 007
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Howland, R. H. (2014). Vagus nerve stimulation. Curr. Behav. Neurosci. Rep. 1, 64–73. doi: 10. 1007/s40473-014-0010-5
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Jacobs, H. I. L., Riphagen, J. M., Razat, C. M., Wiese, S., and Sack, A. T. (2015). Transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation boosts associative memory in older individuals. Neurobiol. Aging 36, 1860–1867. doi: 10. 1016/j. neurobiolaging. 2015. 02. 023
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Jin, Y., and Kong, J. (2017). Transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation: a promising method for treatment of autism spectrum disorders. Front. Neurosci. 10: 609. doi: 10. 3389/fnins. 2016. 00609
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Jongkees, B. J., Immink, M. A., Finisguerra, A., and Colzato, L. S. (2018). Transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation (tVNS) enhances response selection during sequential action. Front. Psychol. 9: 1159. doi: 10. 3389/fpsyg. 2018. 01159
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Kandasamy, N., Garfinkel, S. N., Page, L., Hardy, B., Critchley, H. D., Gurnell, M., et al. (2016). Interoceptive ability predicts survival on a london trading floor. Sci. Rep. 6: 32986. doi: 10. 1038/srep32986
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Kaniusas, E., Kampusch, S., Tittgemeyer, M., Panetsos, F., Gines, R. F., Papa, M., et al. (2019). Current directions in the auricular vagus nerve stimulation i a physiological perspective. Front. Neurosci. 13: 854. doi: 10. 3389/fnins. 2019. 00854
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Karnath, H.-O. (2005). Awareness of the functioning of ones own limbs mediated by the insular cortex? J. Neurosci. 25, 7134–7138. doi: 10. 1523/jneurosci. 1590-05. 2005
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Khalsa, S. S., Adolphs, R., Cameron, O. G., Critchley, H. D., Davenport, P. W., Feinstein, J. S., et al. (2018). Interoception and mental health: a roadmap. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging 3, 501–513. doi: 10. 1016/j. bpsc. 2017. 12. 004
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Khalsa, S. S., Rudrauf, D., Feinstein, J. S., and Tranel, D. (2009). The pathways of interoceptive awareness. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 1494–1496. doi: 10. 1038/nn. 2411
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Klinkenberg, I., Sambeth, A., and Blokland, A. (2011). Acetylcholine and attention. Behav. Brain Res. 221, 430–442. doi: 10. 1016/j. bbr. 2010. 11. 033
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Koo, B., Ham, S. D., Sood, S., and Tarver, B. (2001). Human vagus nerve electrophysiology. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 18, 429–433. doi: 10. 1097/00004691-200109000-00007
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Lange, G., Janal, M. N., Maniker, A., FitzGibbons, J., Fobler, M., Cook, D., et al. (2011). Safety and efficacy of vagus nerve stimulation in fibromyalgia: a phase I/II proof of concept trial. Pain Med. 12, 1406–1413. doi: 10. 1111/j. 1526-4637. 2011. 01203. x
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Lernia, D. D., Serino, S., Cipresso, P., and Riva, G. (2016). Ghosts in the machine. interoceptive modeling for chronic pain treatment. Front. Neurosci. 10: 314. doi: 10. 3389/fnins. 2016. 00314
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Levy, M. L., Levy, K. M., Hoff, D., Amar, A. P., Park, M. S., Conklin, J. M., et al. (2010). Vagus nerve stimulation therapy in patients with autism spectrum disorder and intractable epilepsy: results from the vagus nerve stimulation therapy patient outcome registry. J. Neurosurg. Pediatr. 5, 595–602. doi: 10. 3171/2010. 3. peds09153
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Maniscalco, J. W., and Rinaman, L. (2018). Vagal interoceptive modulation of motivated behavior. Physiology 33, 151–167. doi: 10. 1152/physiol. 00036. 2017
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Manning, K. E., Beresford-Webb, J. A., Aman, L. C. S., Ring, H. A., Watson, P. C., Porges, S. W., et al. (2019). Transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation (t-VNS): a novel effective treatment for temper outbursts in adults with Prader-Willi Syndrome indicated by results from a non-blind study. PLoS One 14: e0223750. doi: 10. 1371/journal. pone. 0223750
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Mariana von Mohr, A. F. (2019). “ The cutaneous borders of interoception: active and social inference of pain and pleasure on the skin,” in The Interoceptive Mind: From Homeostasis to Awareness , eds M. Tsakiris and H. De Preester (Oxford: Oxford University Press).
Marshall, A. C., Gentsch, A., Schröder, L., and Schütz-Bosbach, S. (2018). Cardiac interoceptive learning is modulated by emotional valence perceived from facial expressions. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 13, 677–686. doi: 10. 1093/scan/nsy042
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Martin, C. O., Denburg, N. L., Tranel, D., Granner, M. A., and Bechara, A. (2004). The effects of vagus nerve stimulation on decision-making. Cortex 40, 605–612. doi: 10. 1016/s0010-9452(08)70156-4
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Martino, B. D., Strange, B. A., and Dolan, R. J. (2007). Noradrenergic neuromodulation of human attention for emotional and neutral stimuli. Psychopharmacology 197, 127–136. doi: 10. 1007/s00213-007-1015-5
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Mehling, W., Daubenmier, J., Price, C., and Acree, M. (2013). Self-reported interoceptive awareness in primary care patients with past or current low back pain. J. Pain Res. 6, 403–418. doi: 10. 2147/jpr. s42418
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Melzack, R., Wall, P. D., and Ty, T. C. (1982). Acute pain in an emergency clinic: latency of onset and descriptor patterns related to different injuries. Pain 14, 33–43. doi: 10. 1016/0304-3959(82)90078-1
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Merrill, C. A., Jonsson, M. A. G., Minthon, L., Ejnell, H., Silander, H., C-son Silander, H., et al. (2006). Vagus nerve stimulation in patients with Alzheimers Disease. J. Clin. Psychiatry 67, 1171–1178. doi: 10. 4088/jcp. v67n0801
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Meyer, R. A., Ringkamp, M., Campbell, J. N., and Raja, S. N. (2006). “ Peripheral mechanisms of cutaneous nociception,” in Wall and Melzacks Textbook of Pain , eds S. B. McMahon, M. Koltzenburg, I. Tracey, and D. Turk (Amsterdam: Elsevier), 3–34. doi: 10. 1016/b0-443-07287-6/50006-0
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Moro, V., Pernigo, S., Tsakiris, M., Avesani, R., Edelstyn, N. M. J., Jenkinson, P. M., et al. (2016). Motor versus body awareness: voxel-based lesion analysis in anosognosia for hemiplegia and somatoparaphrenia following right hemisphere stroke. Cortex 83, 62–77. doi: 10. 1016/j. cortex. 2016. 07. 001
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Narayanan, J. T., Watts, R., Haddad, N., Labar, D. R., Li, P. M., and Filippi, C. G. (2002). Cerebral activation during vagus nerve stimulation: a functional MR Study. Epilepsia 43, 1509–1514. doi: 10. 1046/j. 1528-1157. 2002. 16102. x
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Naritoku, D. K., Jensen, R. A., Browning, R. A., Clark, K. B., Smith, D. C., and Terry, R. S. Jr., et al. (2003). Methods for Improving Learning or Memory by Vagus Nerve Stimulation. Washington, DC: U. S. Patent and Trademark Office. US Patent No: US 6, 556, 868 B2. Available online at: https://patents. google. com/patent/US6556868
Noble, L. J., Gonzalez, I. J., Meruva, V. B., Callahan, K. A., Belfort, B. D., Ramanathan, K. R., et al. (2017). Effects of vagus nerve stimulation on extinction of conditioned fear and post-traumatic stress disorder symptoms in rats. Transl. Psychiatry 7: e1217. doi: 10. 1038/tp. 2017. 191
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Nogueira, P. J. C., Tomaz, C., and Williams, C. L. (1994). Contribution of the vagus nerve in mediating the memory-facilitating effects of substance P. Behav. Brain Res. 62, 165–169. doi: 10. 1016/0166-4328(94)90024-8
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Park, H.-D., Bernasconi, F., Salomon, R., Tallon-Baudry, C., Spinelli, L., Seeck, M., et al. (2017). Neural sources and underlying mechanisms of neural responses to heartbeats and their role in bodily self-consciousness: an intracranial EEG Study. Cereb. Cortex 28, 2351–2364. doi: 10. 1093/cercor/bhx136
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Paulus, M. P., and Stein, M. B. (2006). An insular view of anxiety. Biol. Psychiatry 60, 383–387. doi: 10. 1016/j. biopsych. 2006. 03. 042
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Paulus, M. P., and Stein, M. B. (2010). Interoception in anxiety and depression. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 451–463. doi: 10. 1007/s00429-010-0258-9
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Peña, D. F., Childs, J. E., Willett, S., Vital, A., McIntyre, C. K., and Kroener, S. (2014). Vagus nerve stimulation enhances extinction of conditioned fear and modulates plasticity in the pathway from the ventromedial prefrontal cortex to the amygdala. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8: 327. doi: 10. 3389/fnbeh. 2014. 00327
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Peña, D. F., Engineer, N. D., and McIntyre, C. K. (2013). Rapid remission of conditioned fear expression with extinction training paired with vagus nerve stimulation. Biol. Psychiatry 73, 1071–1077. doi: 10. 1016/j. biopsych. 2012. 10. 021
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Petzschner, F. H., Weber, L. A., Gard, T., and Stephan, K. E. (2017). Computational psychosomatics and computational psychiatry: toward a joint framework for differential diagnosis. Biol. Psychiatry 82, 421–430. doi: 10. 1016/j. biopsych. 2017. 05. 012
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Petzschner, F. H., Weber, L. A., Wellstein, K. V., Paolini, G., Do, C. T., and Stephan, K. E. (2019). Focus of attention modulates the heartbeat evoked potential. NeuroImage 186, 595–606. doi: 10. 1016/j. neuroimage. 2018. 11. 037
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Pfeifer, G., Garfinkel, S. N., van Praag, C. D. G., Sahota, K., Betka, S., and Critchley, H. D. (2017). Feedback from the heart: emotional learning and memory is controlled by cardiac cycle interoceptive accuracy and personality. Biol. Psychol. 126, 19–29. doi: 10. 1016/j. biopsycho. 2017. 04. 001
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Pollatos, O., Kirsch, W., and Schandry, R. (2005). On the relationship between interoceptive awareness emotional experience, and brain processes. Cogn. Brain Res. 25, 948–962. doi: 10. 1016/j. cogbrainres. 2005. 09. 019
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Pollatos, O., Kurz, A.-L., Albrecht, J., Schreder, T., Kleemann, A. M., Schöpf, V., et al. (2008). Reduced perception of bodily signals in anorexia nervosa. Eat. Behav. 9, 381–388. doi: 10. 1016/j. eatbeh. 2008. 02. 001
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Pollatos, O., and Schandry, R. (2008). Emotional processing and emotional memory are modulated by interoceptive awareness. Cogn. Emot. 22, 272–287. doi: 10. 1080/02699930701357535
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Quadt, L., Critchley, H. D., and Garfinkel, S. N. (2018). The neurobiology of interoception in health and disease. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1428, 112–128. doi: 10. 1111/nyas. 13915
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Quadt, L., Critchley, H. D., and Garfinkel, S. N. (2019). “ Interoception and emotion: Shared mechanisms and clinical implications,” in The Interoceptive Mind. From Homeostasis to Awareness , eds M. Tsakaris and H. De Preester (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 123–143.
Rong, P., Luo, M., Li, L., Zhang, J., Guo, X., Zhao, B., et al. (2019). Sleep electroencephalography power spectral response to transcutaneous auricular vagus nerve stimulation on insomnia rats. Heart Mind 3, 55–62. doi: 10. 4103/hm. hm_51_19
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Saper, C. B. (2002). The central autonomic nervous system: conscious visceral perception and autonomic pattern generation. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 25, 433–469. doi: 10. 1146/annurev. neuro. 25. 032502. 111311
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Schandry, R. (1981). Heart beat perception and emotional experience. Psychophysiology 18, 483–488. doi: 10. 1111/j. 1469-8986. 1981. tb02486. x
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Sellaro, R., de Gelder, B., Finisguerra, A., and Colzato, L. S. (2018). Transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation (tVNS) enhances recognition of emotions in faces but not bodies. Cortex 99, 213–223. doi: 10. 1016/j. cortex. 2017. 11. 007
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Seth, A. K. (2013). Interoceptive inference emotion, and the embodied self. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 565–573. doi: 10. 1016/j. tics. 2013. 09. 007
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Seth, A. K., Suzuki, K., and Critchley, H. D. (2012). An interoceptive predictive coding model of conscious presence. Front. Psychol. 2: 395. doi: 10. 3389/fpsyg. 2011. 00395
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Seth, A. K., and Tsakiris, M. (2018). Being a beast machine: the somatic basis of selfhood. Trends Cogn. Sci. 22, 969–981. doi: 10. 1016/j. tics. 2018. 08. 008
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Shah, P., Catmur, C., and Bird, G. (2017). From heart to mind: linking interoception emotion, and theory of mind. Cortex 93, 220–223. doi: 10. 1016/j. cortex. 2017. 02. 010
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Shah, P., Hall, R., Catmur, C., and Bird, G. (2016). Alexithymia not autism, is associated with impaired interoception. Cortex 81, 215–220. doi: 10. 1016/j. cortex. 2016. 03. 021
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Sierra, M., and David, A. S. (2011). Depersonalization: a selective impairment of self-awareness. Conscious. Cogn. 20, 99–108. doi: 10. 1016/j. concog. 2010. 10. 018
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Sjogren, M. J. C., Hellstrom, P. T. O., Jonsson, M. A. G., Runnerstam, M., Silander, H. C., and Ben-Menachem, E. (2002). Cognition-enhancing effect of vagus nerve stimulation in patients with Alzheimer’s Disease. J. Clin. Psychiatry 63, 972–980. doi: 10. 4088/jcp. v63n1103
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Souza, R. R., Robertson, N. M., Mathew, E., Tabet, M. N., Bucksot, J. E., Pruitt, D. T., et al. (2020). Efficient parameters of vagus nerve stimulation to enhance extinction learning in an extinction-resistant rat model of PTSD. Prog. Neuro Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 99: 109848. doi: 10. 1016/j. pnpbp. 2019. 109848
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Souza, R. R., Robertson, N. M., Pruitt, D. T., Gonzales, P. A., Hays, S. A., Rennaker, R. L., et al. (2019). Vagus nerve stimulation reverses the extinction impairments in a model of PTSD with prolonged and repeated trauma. Stress 22, 509–520. doi: 10. 1080/10253890. 2019. 1602604
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Szeska, C., Richter, J., Wendt, J., Weymar, M., and Hamm, A. O. (2020). Promoting long-term inhibition of human fear responses by non-invasive transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation during extinction training. Sci. Rep. 10: 1529. doi: 10. 1038/s41598-020-58412-w
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Talley, C. P., Clayborn, H., Jewel, E., McCarty, R., and Gold, P. E. (2002). Vagotomy attenuates effects of l-glucose but not of d-glucose on spontaneous alternation performance. Physiol. Behav. 77, 243–249. doi: 10. 1016/s0031-9384(02)00850-8
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Terasawa, Y., Moriguchi, Y., Tochizawa, S., and Umeda, S. (2014). Interoceptive sensitivity predicts sensitivity to the emotions of others. Cogn. Emot. 28, 1435–1448. doi: 10. 1080/02699931. 2014. 888988
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Terhaar, J., Viola, F. C., Bär, K.-J., and Debener, S. (2012). Heartbeat evoked potentials mirror altered body perception in depressed patients. Clin. Neurophysiol. 123, 1950–1957. doi: 10. 1016/j. clinph. 2012. 02. 086
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Van den Bergh, O. V., Witthöft, M., Petersen, S., and Brown, R. J. (2017). Symptoms and the body: taking the inferential leap. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 74, 185–203. doi: 10. 1016/j. neubiorev. 2017. 01. 015
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Villani, V., Tsakiris, M., and Azevedo, R. T. (2019). Transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation improves interoceptive accuracy. Neuropsychologia 134: 107201. doi: 10. 1016/j. neuropsychologia. 2019. 107201
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Vonck, K., Raedt, R., Naulaerts, J., Vogelaere, F. D., Thiery, E., Roost, D. V., et al. (2014). Vagus nerve stimulation…25 years later! What do we know about the effects on cognition? Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 45, 63–71. doi: 10. 1016/j. neubiorev. 2014. 05. 005
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Werner, N. S., Jung, K., Duschek, S., and Schandry, R. (2009). Enhanced cardiac perception is associated with benefits in decision-making. Psychophysiology 46, 1123–1129. doi: 10. 1111/j. 1469-8986. 2009. 00855. x
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Werner, N. S., Peres, I., Duschek, S., and Schandry, R. (2010). Implicit memory for emotional words is modulated by cardiac perception. Biol. Psychol. 85, 370–376. doi: 10. 1016/j. biopsycho. 2010. 08. 008
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Wernicke, J. F., Terry, R. S. Jr., and Baker, R. G. (1993). Treatment of Eating Disorders by Nerve Stimulation. Washington, DC: U. S. Patent and Trademark Office. US Patent No 5, 104. Available online at: https://patents. google. com/patent/US5263480A/en
Wiens, S., Mezzacappa, E. S., and Katkin, E. S. (2000). Heartbeat detection and the experience of emotions. Cogn. Emot. 14, 417–427. doi: 10. 1080/026999300378905
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Williams, C. L., and Jensen, R. A. (1993). Effects of vagotomy on leu-enkephalin-induced changes in memory storage processes. Physiol. Behav. 54, 659–663. doi: 10. 1016/0031-9384(93)90073-o
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Yoris, A., García, A. M., Salamone, P. C., Sedeño, L., García-Cordero, I., and Ibáñez, A. (2019). “ Cardiac interoception in neurological conditions and its relevance for dimensional approaches,” in Interoceptive Mind: From Homeostasis to Awareness , eds M. Tsakaris and H. De Preester (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 197–211.