1,467
11
Essay, 8 pages (2000 words)

Food craving: new contributions on its assessment, moderators, and consequences

Food craving refers to an intense desire to consume a specific food. In Western societies, these foods usually have high palatability and are energy dense, that is, they have high sugar and/or fat content. Food craving is a multidimensional experience as it includes cognitive (e. g., thinking about food), emotional (e. g., desire to eat or changes in mood), behavioral (e. g., seeking and consuming food), and physiological (e. g., salivation) aspects ( Nederkoorn et al., 2000 ; Cepeda-Benito et al., 2000a ).

Subjective self-report appears to be the most viable method for the assessment of craving as other measurement modalities (e. g., peripheral autonomic responses) typically suffer from a lack of specificity ( Shiffman, 2000 ). The most often used instruments are the Food Cravings Questionnaires (FCQs; Cepeda-Benito et al., 2000a , b ). Momentary food craving can be measured with a 15-item state version (FCQ-S) while the frequency of food craving experiences can be measured with a 39-item trait version (FCQ-T), which contains nine subscales. However, factor structure could not be replicated in some studies and as the FCQ-T usually has very high internal consistency, researchers often report its total score only. Consequently, such a long measure may not be necessary in order to assess a general index of trait food craving. In the current research topic, Meule et al. (2014) present a reduced version of the German FCQ-T (FCQ-T-r), which consists of 15 items only. Results showed that correlates of this short version were similar to those that have been found for the long version, for example, that FCQ-T scores are able to predict food-cue induced craving ( Meule et al., 2012a , b ). Following up on this, Rodríguez-Martín and Molerio-Pérez (2014) also could not replicate the nine-factorial structure of the Spanish FCQ-T ( Cepeda-Benito et al., 2000b ). They could demonstrate, however, that scores of the Spanish FCQ-T-r were highly correlated with scores of the long version as well as with the 24 excluded items. Moreover, it was shown that validity indices were similar for both versions, providing further support that the FCQ-T-r represents an adequate alternative for the assessment of trait food craving as measured by the FCQ-T.

Animal models are an important part of research on eating behavior. While people can be asked if they experience a desire to eat a food, measuring food craving in animals is not straightforward. Following abstinence from sugar, rats will exhibit a larger binge than ever before, which may be interpreted as an experience of craving ( Avena et al., 2005 ). A rather indirect measure of food craving may be sensory-specific satiety (SST; specifically, the lack thereof). It refers to a temporary decline in food liking and food wanting derived from consuming a certain food in comparison to other unconsumed foods ( Havermans et al., 2009 ). Reichelt et al. (2014) present a study in which rats consumed a so-called cafeteria diet of palatable, high-calorie foods for 2 weeks. They found that these rats showed impaired SST following consumption of a high-calorie solution, which may suggest that exposure to obesogenic diets impacts upon neurocircuitry involved in motivated control of eating behavior.

The ingestion of food is associated with a rewarding consequence and, thus, the incentive value of that particular food increases and its sensory attributes become signals for satisfaction ( Havermans, 2013 ). Therefore, through Pavlovian conditioning, exposure to food-cues can likely trigger food craving. In experimental research, this can easily be examined by presenting pictorial food stimuli on a computer screen while psychophysiological data or subjective ratings are recorded. However, food image sets vary considerably across laboratories and image characteristics and food composition are often unspecified. Moreover, study results may be adversely affected by confounding variables such as perceptual characteristics of the stimuli. To remedy this, Blechert et al. (2014) developed a comprehensive database of food and non-food images along with detailed meta-data. This database will facilitate standardization and comparability across studies and will advance experimental research on food craving and eating behavior as it enables to match and control stimulus sets on a range of important variables.

Exposure to tempting food-cues, however, does not trigger food craving or lead to food consumption in each and every situation. Grubliauskiene and Dewitte (2014), for example, show that boys actually ate fewer candies following an unobtrusive pre-exposure to candies as compared to when there was no pre-exposure. Unexpectedly, however, this effect could not be shown for girls. Moreover, as was firstly demonstrated by Morewedge et al. (2010), instead of inducing food craving (i. e., having a sensitizing effect), repeatedly imagining the consumption of a food leads people to habituate to it and consequently reduces consumption of that food. Missbach et al. (2014) were able to replicate this finding with different food items than were used in the original studies. Importantly, they found that this habituation effect was neutralized by self-regulatory depletion. That is, repeated imagination of food consumption only reduces subsequent food intake when self-regulatory resources are available.

Research consistently demonstrates gender differences in food craving: women are more likely to experience food cravings than men ( Weingarten and Elston, 1991 ). It is tempting to assume that these differences are related to hormonal differences between women and men, particularly as many women experience increases in food cravings perimenstrually and prenatally ( Hormes, 2014 ). However, research on these experiences is scarce. Orloff and Hormes (2014) review the available literature on food cravings during pregnancy. They challenge the notion that perimenstrual or prenatal food craving is associated with hormonal changes, but suggest that cultural and psychosocial factors are more important determinants of food craving experiences during pregnancy and of excess gestational weight gain.

Food craving can occur as a result of specific mood states (often negative mood) and is marked by anticipation of mood enhancing effects of food intake ( Cepeda-Benito et al., 2000a ). Food craving is also associated with external eating, that is, it is often triggered by cues in the environment. Pfattheicher and Sassenrath (2014) report that emotional eating is positively related to individual differences in prevention focus while external eating is positively related to individual differences in promotion focus. Hence, this study showed that trait-like self-regulatory orientations are differentially related to specific eating styles, which may inspire intervention approaches for the reduction of food craving.

Potenza and Grilo (2014) briefly summarize contemporary research on food craving such as its neuronal underpinnings. They also highlight its relevance in obesity and binge eating disorder and suggest that research on and therapy of these disorders may benefit from providing an addiction framework. For instance, some approaches that effectively target drug cravings have also been shown to reduce food cravings. In a similar vein, Davis et al. (2014) investigated the effects of a methylphenidate challenge in individuals exhibiting addiction-like eating behavior. Individuals with “ food addiction” reported more intense food craving than controls and were resistant to the food intake suppression that is typically induced by dopamine agonists. This supports that compulsive overeating is related to increased food craving and dopamine signaling-strength differences.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank all authors, editors and reviewers who have worked thoroughly in order to provide these high-quality contributions in a timely manner.

References

Avena, N. M., Long, K. A., and Hoebel, B. G. (2005). Sugar-dependent rats show enhanced responding for sugar after abstinence: evidence of a sugar deprivation effect. Physiol. Behav . 84, 359–362. doi: 10. 1016/j. physbeh. 2004. 12. 016

|||

Blechert, J., Meule, A., Busch, N. A., and Ohla, K. (2014). Food-pics: an image database for experimental research on eating and appetite. Front. Psychol . 5: 617. doi: 10. 3389/fpsyg. 2014. 00617

|||

Cepeda-Benito, A., Gleaves, D. H., Fernández, M. C., Vila, J., Williams, T. L., and Reynoso, J. (2000b). The development and validation of Spanish versions of the state and trait food cravings questionnaires. Behav. Res. Ther . 38, 1125–1138. doi: 10. 1016/S0005-7967(99)00141-2

|||

Cepeda-Benito, A., Gleaves, D. H., Williams, T. L., and Erath, S. A. (2000a). The development and validation of the state and trait food-cravings questionnaires. Behav. Ther . 31, 151–173. doi: 10. 1016/S0005-7894(00)80009-X

|||

Davis, C., Levitan, R. D., Kaplan, A. S., Kennedy, J. L., and Carter, J. C. (2014). Food cravings, appetite, and snack-food consumption in response to a psychomotor stimulant drug: the moderating effect of “ food-addiction.” Front. Psychol . 5: 403. doi: 10. 3389/fpsyg. 2014. 00403

|||

Grubliauskiene, A., and Dewitte, S. (2014). Temptation in the background: non-consummatory exposure to food temptation enhances self-regulation in boys but not in girls. Front. Psychol . 5: 788. doi: 10. 3389/fpsyg. 2014. 00788

|||

Havermans, R. C. (2013). Pavlovian craving and overeating: a conditioned incentive model. Curr. Obes. Rep . 2, 165–170. doi: 10. 1007/s13679-013-0053-z

|

Havermans, R. C., Janssen, T., Giesen, J. C. A. H., Roefs, A., and Jansen, A. (2009). Food liking, food wanting, and sensory-specific satiety. Appetite 52, 222–225. doi: 10. 1016/j. appet. 2008. 09. 020

|||

Hormes, J. M. (2014). “ Perimenstrual chocolate craving: from pharmacology and physiology to cognition and culture,” in Handbook of Diet and Nutrition in the Menstrual Cycle, Periconception and Fertility , eds C. J. Hollins-Martin, O. B. A. van den Akker, C. R. Martin, and V. R. Preedy (Wageningen: Wageningen Academic Publishers), 137–153.

Meule, A., Hermann, T., and Kübler, A. (2014). A short version of the Food Cravings Questionnaire – Trait : the FCQ-T-reduced. Front. Psychol . 5: 190. doi: 10. 3389/fpsyg. 2014. 00190

|||

Meule, A., Lutz, A., Vögele, C., and Kübler, A. (2012a). Food cravings discriminate differentially between successful and unsuccessful dieters and non-dieters. Validation of the Food Craving Questionnaires in German. Appetite 58, 88–97. doi: 10. 1016/j. appet. 2011. 09. 010

|||

Meule, A., Skirde, A. K., Freund, R., Vögele, C., and Kübler, A. (2012b). High-calorie food-cues impair working memory performance in high and low food cravers. Appetite 59, 264–269. doi: 10. 1016/j. appet. 2012. 05. 010

|||

Missbach, B., Florack, A., Weissmann, L., and König, J. (2014). Mental imagery interventions reduce subsequent food intake only when self-regulatory resources are available. Front. Psychol . 5: 1391. doi: 10. 3389/fpsyg. 2014. 01391

|||

Morewedge, C. K., Eun Huh, Y., and Vosgerau, J. (2010). Thought for food: imagined consumption reduces actual consumption. Science 330, 1530–1533. doi: 10. 1126/science. 1195701

|||

Nederkoorn, C., Smulders, F. T. Y., and Jansen, A. (2000). Cephalic phase responses, craving and food intake in normal subjects. Appetite 35, 45–55. doi: 10. 1006/appe. 2000. 0328

|||

Orloff, N. C., and Hormes, J. M. (2014). Pickles and ice cream! Food cravings in pregnancy: hypotheses, preliminary evidence, and directions for future research. Front. Psychol . 5: 1076. doi: 10. 3389/fpsyg. 2014. 01076

|||

Pfattheicher, S., and Sassenrath, C. (2014). A regulatory focus perspective on eating behavior: how prevention and promotion focus relates to emotional, external, and restrained eating. Front. Psychol . 5: 1314. doi: 10. 3389/fpsyg. 2014. 01314

|||

Potenza, M. N., and Grilo, C. (2014). How relevant is food craving to obesity and its treatment? Front. Psychiatry 5: 164. doi: 10. 3389/fpsyt. 2014. 00164

|||

Reichelt, A. C., Morris, M. J., and Westbrook, R. F. (2014). Cafeteria diet impairs expression of sensory-specific satiety and stimulus-outcome learning. Front. Psychol . 5: 852. doi: 10. 3389/fpsyg. 2014. 00852

|||

Rodríguez-Martín, B. C., and Molerio-Pérez, O. (2014). Exploring the factor structure of the food cravings questionnaire-trait in cuban adults. Front. Psychol . 5: 214. doi: 10. 3389/fpsyg. 2014. 00214

||

Shiffman, S. (2000). Comments on cravings. Addiction 95(Suppl. 2), S171–S175. doi: 10. 1080/09652140050111744

|||

Weingarten, H. P., and Elston, D. (1991). Food cravings in a college population. Appetite 17, 167–175. doi: 10. 1016/0195-6663(91)90019-O

|||

Thank's for Your Vote!
Food craving: new contributions on its assessment, moderators, and consequences. Page 1
Food craving: new contributions on its assessment, moderators, and consequences. Page 2
Food craving: new contributions on its assessment, moderators, and consequences. Page 3
Food craving: new contributions on its assessment, moderators, and consequences. Page 4
Food craving: new contributions on its assessment, moderators, and consequences. Page 5
Food craving: new contributions on its assessment, moderators, and consequences. Page 6
Food craving: new contributions on its assessment, moderators, and consequences. Page 7
Food craving: new contributions on its assessment, moderators, and consequences. Page 8
Food craving: new contributions on its assessment, moderators, and consequences. Page 9

This work, titled "Food craving: new contributions on its assessment, moderators, and consequences" was written and willingly shared by a fellow student. This sample can be utilized as a research and reference resource to aid in the writing of your own work. Any use of the work that does not include an appropriate citation is banned.

If you are the owner of this work and don’t want it to be published on AssignBuster, request its removal.

Request Removal
Cite this Essay

References

AssignBuster. (2022) 'Food craving: new contributions on its assessment, moderators, and consequences'. 16 August.

Reference

AssignBuster. (2022, August 16). Food craving: new contributions on its assessment, moderators, and consequences. Retrieved from https://assignbuster.com/food-craving-new-contributions-on-its-assessment-moderators-and-consequences/

References

AssignBuster. 2022. "Food craving: new contributions on its assessment, moderators, and consequences." August 16, 2022. https://assignbuster.com/food-craving-new-contributions-on-its-assessment-moderators-and-consequences/.

1. AssignBuster. "Food craving: new contributions on its assessment, moderators, and consequences." August 16, 2022. https://assignbuster.com/food-craving-new-contributions-on-its-assessment-moderators-and-consequences/.


Bibliography


AssignBuster. "Food craving: new contributions on its assessment, moderators, and consequences." August 16, 2022. https://assignbuster.com/food-craving-new-contributions-on-its-assessment-moderators-and-consequences/.

Work Cited

"Food craving: new contributions on its assessment, moderators, and consequences." AssignBuster, 16 Aug. 2022, assignbuster.com/food-craving-new-contributions-on-its-assessment-moderators-and-consequences/.

Get in Touch

Please, let us know if you have any ideas on improving Food craving: new contributions on its assessment, moderators, and consequences, or our service. We will be happy to hear what you think: [email protected]