1,383
27
Essay, 14 pages (3500 words)

Individual differences and psychosis-risk screening: practical suggestions to improve the scope and quality of early identification

Overview

Research related to identifying people at clinical high-risk (CHR) for psychosis has seen exponential growth in the past decade, in part fueled by the building evidence that intervention during this phase can prevent, delay, and/or lessen the severity of future negative outcomes ( 1 , 2 ). Significant gains in identification include increased accuracy using risk calculators based on large samples and advanced statistical approaches ( 3 5 ). Additionally, this body of work has revealed a clinically relevant vulnerability in those at CHR—associated with high rates of substance abuse, trauma exposure, cognitive impairments, and suicidal risk ( 6 , 7 )—irrespective of transition to first episode or full-threshold psychosis ( 8 ). Findings like these have likely generated a number of recognized and heretofore undiscovered benefits relating to earlier interventions, stronger therapeutic relationships, and shorter duration of untreated psychosis. To build on these gains it is important to consider that while the current CHR assessment approach has relatively adequate positive predictive value in help-seeking individuals, it fails to capture a clinically meaningful percentage of individuals truly at risk for psychosis, and concurrently might over-pathologize individuals who are not at risk. Unfortunately, typical CHR interview practices such as those employed with the Structured Interview for Prodromal Syndromes (SIPS) ( 9 ) in North America, may in some cases not fully honor individual differences of those being evaluated for CHR.

In this Perspective commentary, we highlight several issues with this “ one-size-fits-all” approach in relation to the ethnic and racial differences as well as culture, context and socio-economic status. This has a clear relevance for this special issue on identifying individuals at CHR in different cultures and countries, as we highlight how differences in factors such as race/ethnicity and culture can substantially impact clinician-ascribed diagnostic ratings, even within a single country—a point clearly aligning with the broader volume focusing on cross-cultural and international differences in CHR research. We also discuss how the status quo approach of interviews such as the SIPS’ identification of Attenuated Psychosis Syndrome (APS) and related risk states can limit the incorporation of important information relating to comorbidity and developmental considerations, relevant concerns in the CHR population. After highlighting issues for each area, we will discuss suggestions for current research and intervention work, as well as outline a series of goals for future studies in this domain.

Cultural and Contextual Competence

Evidence suggests that immigrants, ethnic/racial minorities, and those raised in certain urban environments are at a heightened risk for developing psychosis-spectrum disorders ( 10 , 11 ). It is possible that greater exposure to risk factors for psychosis, including trauma and discrimination associated with any minority identity (e. g., race, sexual identity, gender), lead to higher rates of psychosis symptoms and diagnoses ( 12 ). At the same time, it may be that contextual or environmental factors can lead to endorsing items—particularly those related to suspiciousness—on CHR assessment tools when the underlying mechanism is either distally connected or unrelated to psychosis ( 13 , 14 ). Responses to discrimination, crime, and/or trauma may be causally, concurrently, concurrently that leads to causality, or illusorily linked to psychosis- risk symptoms ( 13 , 14 ). Further, clinician biases can result in systematically ascribing psychosis-spectrum explanations for culturally distinct behaviors ( 15 ). For instance, some common themes in CHR interviews and screening tools probe for very normative behaviors in certain cultures (e. g., belief in superstitions, déjà vu, having special talents, religious convictions). In some cases, endorsement of these more normative prompts is associated with better functioning ( 16 ). All of these factors can lead to diagnostic confusion, false-positives, and ultimately large-scale health disparities for minority groups.

There are several possible routes to addressing these issues should they arise as consistent concerns in the field. First, it is useful to ensure that interview techniques are sensitive to cultural factors, which may require using structured interviews and potentially modifying existing measures and processes to explicitly probe for these relations. Assessments should allow time for clients to share their individual and cultural views around what are intended as CHR probes, such as their possible experiences of discrimination, social deprivation, and/or trauma related to their surrounding neighborhood context ( 15 , 17 ). Additionally, screening tools used to indicate risk, and often trigger referrals, should be validated in different cultures and with different racial and minority groups; results should be considered accordingly prior to assuming psychopathology ( 16 ). Such analyses could drill down to scale, factor, or the item level. More broadly, designing validation studies to understand the role of other, often related, aspects of identity (e. g., SES, religion/spirituality, cultural identification, help-seeking response style, language differences) can create more individualized approaches to risk assessment. More explicitly infusing cultural competency into risk assessment training (e. g., the role of clinician bias, socially mediated stress as a dynamic factor when establishing risk), and perhaps even empirically measuring assessor’s cultural competence may begin to create a more sensitive and possibly accurate workforce ( 18 ). These steps may help reduce the risk of misdiagnosis as well as enhance detection in those who may be more vulnerable for risk of a psychotic disorder.

Comorbidity

Comorbid psychopathology is another key individual difference requiring consideration. In many cases, “ CHR symptoms” may more accurately reflect other, non-psychotic psychopathological processes. For example, symptoms of OCD such as recurring thoughts may in some cases resemble unusual thought content in CHR (e. g., “ Have you felt that you are not in control of your own ideas or thoughts?”), despite being presumed as clinically “ distinct.” Additionally, use of psychoactive substances can elicit psychotic-like experiences that persist beyond acute intoxication and therefore be misinterpreted as risk symptoms despite resolving with sustained abstinence. Likewise, the persistent and preoccupying cognitive distortions associated with false perceptions of body image in eating disorders and body dysmorphic disorder can often resemble the delusional thinking and perceptual disturbances experienced in populations at clinical high risk for psychosis ( 19 ). Differentiating all of these experiences from “ risk for psychosis” vis-a-vis CHR can present challenges, particularly because comorbid health conditions, experiences of adversity, and substance use are not only risk factors for CHR and psychosis, but can also mimic psychosis-risk and appear in conjunction with the CHR state, each of which has different clinical implications ( 20 23 ).

There are some practical solutions to addressing comorbidity in those at CHR, should additional research identify comorbidity as a concern for accurate identification of those at risk. Assessing possible contributors to symptoms at the same depth as psychosis risk, in recognition of clinician bias to one’s own specialty, can help address whether factors such as trauma, substance use, or eating disorders for instance are contributors, comorbidities, or unrelated to psychosis risk. In many cases, comorbidities can have accompanying psychotic symptoms, suggesting a need in some cases to expand the definition of psychosis in the context of CHR conceptualizations. Additionally, graduate training and continuing education programs providing psychodiagnostic training, periodic re-training, and assessment validity check-ins may limit misdiagnosis or possible assessor drift. Truly attending to these concerns may require more frequent and in-depth follow-up and an openness to new information that may run contrary to initial impressions—efforts that will hopefully provide a more accurate and individualized evaluation. Finally, it is important to recognize that comorbidity is the rule rather than the exception in individuals at CHR. Although CHR status does not appear to reliably predict other outcomes beyond psychosis ( 24 ), certainly the rich information regarding comorbidity will serve to strengthen predictive models and relatedly help to better characterize individual variation and thereby promote precision medicine. Notably, these conditions are clinically relevant in their own right, and should be carefully considered and treated in this manner.

Development

A similar call for developmental considerations can also be made. For instance, pre-adolescents may endorse over valued beliefs (e. g., “ Do you daydream a lot or find yourself preoccupied with stories, fantasies, or ideas?”), and adolescents may endorse ideas of reference (e. g., “ Have you had the sense that you are often the center of people’s attention?”), when such “ symptoms” at face-value are often normative in these age groups ( 25 , 26 ). Younger individuals might also interpret interview questions in ways that are different from the interviewer’s intention or respond in all-or-nothing extremes on self-report measures ( 26 32 ). Longitudinal studies of self-reported psychotic-like experiences reveal a declining rate of endorsement with age, suggesting that these experiences may be part of typical developmental processes. Incorporating developmental awareness in our measures and among clinicians will likely increase accuracy of CHR detection ( 33 , 34 ). For example, it is quite easy for CHR diagnosticians to confuse sensitivity to sound and belief in an invisible audience (experiences that become increasingly sensitive or salient in adolescence) with psychotic experiences ( 25 , 35 , 36 ). Measures of functioning are also likely particularly sensitive to stages of development as well; the functional expectations of adolescents and young adults vary dramatically from year to year.

There are also a number of practical suggestions to address issues around developmental variation in CHR research and treatment, should development be identified as a reliable confound to accurate identification. First, a developmentally-informed conceptualization of risk can be achieved by training clinicians on the unique developmental considerations of this age range, creating anchors within interview tools that reflect typical and atypical development, developing age-informed norms/cutoff scores, maintaining a sensitivity to response style biases, thoroughly probing endorsements to ensure a shared understanding of meaning, and committing to a longitudinal approach (clinically and through research). Second, investigators and clinicians alike can stay current on not only the literature (as we are regularly discovering new potential developmental confounds in this area), but also norms for adolescent behaviors [e. g., around social media use, social engagement patterns, and dating; ( 37 , 38 )] and recent trends in subculture identification and practice (a rapidly shifting area with many potential nuances that would likely confound accurate assessment and treatment). Further, investigators can be mindful not to treat the adolescent period as a unidimensional construct, but rather, understand that this is a dynamic span of time, beginning just at the end of late childhood and carrying many individuals into the mid to late 20’s. The scope of “ normative” behavior, as well as social and role functioning expectations will be much easier to assess with that consideration in mind, and to this point, it may be best to view different stages by the attainment of developmentally relevant landmarks, instead of age or year in school.

Conclusions

Prevention efforts in psychosis have never been more promising. True prevention will require the CHR concept to expand beyond specialty clinics, perhaps creating meaningful—but not insurmountable—hurdles between the current state of affairs and the aspirations of identifying the large proportion of people at CHR who are currently undetected, and avoiding labeling people as at-risk who are not. Systemic issues, such as increasing education, creating a culture of hope, prevention, and recovery, and reducing stigma so that more people are willing to seek help, are essential to this goal. Other systemic issues include addressing the bifurcation of mental health and education systems relevant to the risk age, engaging community partners beyond mental health specialists, and considering a more pluripotent approach to identification whereby outcomes are more broadly defined beyond the presence of psychosis. Within-field innovations are required as well, such as creating interview tools with increased accuracy and that require less training, and considering mechanisms when building risk models. To reach these goals, we will benefit from methods that require substantial investments, such as diagnostically-fluid prospective studies and studies that incorporate the voices of people at CHR and their families (e. g., participatory action research).

Complementing these initiatives are the promise of contextual and cultural adaptations. In the diagnosis of illnesses with threshold psychosis, for instance, when using the DSM-5 cultural formulation interview to re-evaluate diagnoses in an ethnically diverse sample initially diagnosed by community providers, Adeponle et al. ( 17 ) reported that 49% of individuals with initial diagnoses of psychosis were changed to non-psychotic disorders, while only 5% of initial non-psychotic disorders were re-diagnosed as having a psychotic disorder. In the emerging CHR field we argue a similar need to reach beyond nomothetic and normative perspectives, and to peer deeper to consider contextual and individualized approaches to identification and care.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (grants R01MH112612, and R34MH110506 to JS; R01MH112545 to VM and R01MH112613 to LE) and the Maryland Department of Health and Mental Hygiene, Behavioral Health Administration through the Center for Excellence on Early Intervention for Serious Mental Illness (OPASS# 14-13717G/M00B4400241 to JS).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work would not be possible without the tireless efforts of our research teams. A special thanks is in order for Caroline Roemer for her help with editing and formatting. Most importantly, we would like to thank all of the families and participants who partner with us in our research.

References

1. van der Gaag M, Smit F, Bechdolf A, French P, Linszen DH, Yung AR, et al. Preventing a first episode of psychosis: meta-analysis of randomized controlled prevention trials of 12 month and longer-term follow-ups. Schizophr Res.(2013) 149: 56–62. doi: 10. 1016/j. schres. 2013. 07. 004

|

2. Okuzawa N, Kline E, Fuertes J, Negi S, Reeves G, Himelhoch S, et al. Psychotherapy for adolescents and young adults at high risk for psychosis: a systematic review. Early Interv Psychiatry (2014) 8: 307–22. doi: 10. 1111/eip. 12129

||

3. Cannon TD, Yu C, Addington J, Bearden CE, Cadenhead KS, Cornblatt BA, et al. An individualized risk calculator for research in prodromal psychosis. Am J Psychiatry (2016) 173: 980–8. doi: 10. 1176/appi. ajp. 2016. 15070890

||

4. Carrion RE, Demmin D, Auther AM, McLaughlin D, Olsen R, Lencz T, et al. Duration of attenuated positive and negative symptoms in individuals at clinical high risk: associations with risk of conversion to psychosis and functional outcome. J Psychiatr Res.(2016) 81: 95–101. doi: 10. 1016/j. jpsychires. 2016. 06. 021

||

5. Ciarleglio AJ, Brucato G, Masucci MD, Altschuler R, Colibazzi T, Corcoran CM, et al. A predictive model for conversion to psychosis in clinical high-risk patients. Psychol Med.(2018) 1–10. doi: 10. 1017/S003329171800171X

||

6. Fusar-Poli P, Borgwardt S, Bechdolf A, Addington J, Riecher-Rossler A, Schultze-Lutter F, et al. The psychosis high-risk state: a comprehensive state-of-the-art review. JAMA Psychiatry (2013) 70: 107–20 doi: 10. 1001/jamapsychiatry. 2013. 269

||

7. Kraan T, Velthorst E, Smit F, de Haan L, van der Gaag M. Trauma and recent life events in individuals at ultra high risk for psychosis: review and meta-analysis. Schizophr Res.(2015) 161: 143–9. doi: 10. 1016/j. schres. 2014. 11. 026

||

8. Lin A, Wood SJ, Nelson B, Beaven A, McGorry P, Yung A. Outcomes of nontransitioned cases in a sample at ultra-high risk for psychosis. Am J Psychiatry (2015) 172: 249–58. doi: 10. 1176/appi. ajp. 2014. 13030418

||

9. Miller TJ, McGlashan TH, Woods SW, Stein K, Driesen N, Corcoran CM, et al. Symptom assessment in schizophrenic prodromal states. Psychiatr Q.(1999) 70: 273–87. doi: 10. 1023/A: 1022034115078

||

10. Radua J, Ramella-Cravaro V, Ioannidis JP, Reichenberg A, Phiphopthatsanee N, Amir T, et al. What causes psychosis? An umbrella review of risk and protective factors. World Psychiatry (2018) 17: 49–66. doi: 10. 1002/wps. 20490

||

11. van Os J, Kenis G, Rutten BP. The environment and schizophrenia. Nature (2010) 468: 203–12. doi: 10. 1038/nature09563

||

12. Selten J, van Os J, Cantor-Graae E. The social defeat hypothesis of schizophrenia: issues of measurement and reverse causality. World Psychiatry (2016) 15: 294–95. doi: 10. 1002/wps. 20369

||

13. Kline E, Millman ZB, Denenny D, Wilson C, Thompson E, Demro C, et al. Trauma and psychosis symptoms in a sample of help-seeking youth. Schizophr Res.(2016) 175: 174–9. doi: 10. 1016/j. schres. 2016. 04. 006

||

14. Wilson C, Smith ME, Thompson E, Demro C, Kline E, Bussell K, et al. Context matters: the impact of neighborhood crime and paranoid symptoms on psychosis risk assessment. Schizophr Res.(2016) 171: 56–61. doi: 10. 1016/j. schres. 2016. 01. 007

||

15. Olbert CM, Nagendra A, Buck B. Meta-analysis of Black vs. White racial disparity in schizophrenia diagnosis in the United States: do structured assessments attenuate racial disparities? J Abnorm Psychol.(2018) 127: 104–15. doi: 10. 1037/abn0000309

||

16. Schiffman J, Rakhshan P, Millman Z, Andorko N. Contextual Considerations in assessing clinical high-risk status. In: Symposium Presented at the Annual Meeting for the Society for Research in Psychopathology . Indianapolis, IN (2018).

17. Adeponle AB, Thombs BD, Groleau D, Jarvis E, Kirmayer LJ. Using the cultural formulation to resolve uncertainty in diagnoses of psychosis among ethnoculturally diverse patients. Psychiatr Serv.(2012) 63: 147–53. doi: 10. 1176/appi. ps. 201100280

||

18. Alegria M, Atkins M, Farmer E, Slaton E, Stelk W. One size does not fit all: taking diversity, culture and context seriously. Adm Policy Ment Health (2010) 37: 48–60. doi: 10. 1007/s10488-010-0283-2

|

19. Foulon C. Schizophrenia and eating disorders. L’Encephale (2003) 29: 463–6. doi: 10. 1016/S0151-9638(18)31205-5

||

20. Buchy L, Cannon TD, Anticevic A, Lyngberg K, Cadenhead KS, Cornblatt BA, et al. Evaluating the impact of cannabis use on thalamic connectivity in youth at clinical high risk of psychosis. BMC Psychiatry (2015) 15: 276. doi: 10. 1186/s12888-015-0656-x

||

21. Carney R, Cotter J, Firth J, Bradshaw T, Yung AR. Cannabis use and symptom severity in individuals at ultra high risk for psychosis: a meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatr Scand.(2017) 136: 5–15. doi: 10. 1111/acps. 12699

||

22. Miotto P, Pollini B, Restaneo A, Favaretto G, Sisti D, Rocchi MB, et al. Symptoms of psychosis in anorexia and bulimia nervosa. Psychiatry Res.(2010) 175: 237–43. doi: 10. 1016/j. psychres. 2009. 03. 011

||

23. Kraan T, Van Dam DS, Velthorst E, de Ruigh EL, Nieman DH, Durston S, et al. Childhood trauma and clinical outcome in patients at ultra-high risk of transition to psychosis. Schizophr Res.(2015) 169: 193–8. doi: 10. 1016/j. schres. 2015. 10. 030

||

24. Woods SW, Powers AR III, Taylor JH, Davidson CA, Johannesen JK, Addington J, et al. Lack of diagnostic pluripotentiality in patients at clinical high risk for psychosis: specificity of comorbidity persistence and search for pluripotential subgroups. Schizophr Bull.(2018) 44: 254–63. doi: 10. 1093/schbul/sbx138

||

25. Carol E, Mittal VA. Normative adolescent experiences may confound assessment of positive symptoms in youth at ultra-high risk for psychosis. Schizophr Res.(2015) 166: 358–9. doi: 10. 1016/j. schres. 2015. 04. 043

|

26. Brandizzi M, Schultze-Lutter F, Masillo A, Lanna A, Curto M, Lindau JF, et al. Self-reported attenuated psychotic-like experiences in help-seeking adolescents and their association with age, functioning and psychopathology. Schizophr Res.(2014) 160: 110–7. doi: 10. 1016/j. schres. 2014. 10. 005

||

27. Bartels-Velthius AA, van de Willige G, Jenner JA, van Os J, Wiersma D. Course of auditory vocal hallucinations in childhood: 5-year follow-up study. Br J Psychiatry (2011) 199: 296–302. doi: 10. 1192/bjp. bp. 110. 086918

|

28. Kelleher I, Connor D, Clarke MC, Devlin N, Harley M, Cannon M. Prevalence of psychotic symptoms in childhood and adolescence: a systematic review and meta-analysis of population-based studies. Psychol Med.(2012) 42: 1857–63. doi: 10. 1017/S0033291711002960

||

29. Laurens KR, Hodgins S, Maughan B, Murray RM, Rutter ML, Taylor EA. Community screening for psychotic-like experiences and other putative antecedents of schizophrenia in children aged 9-12 258 years. Schizophr Res.(2007) 90: 130–46. doi: 10. 1016/j. schres. 2006. 11. 006

|

30. Rubio G, Marin-Lozano J, Ferre F, Martinez-Gras I, Rodriguez146-Jimenez R, Sanz J, et al. Psychopathologic differences between cannabis-induced psychoses and recent-onset primary psychoses with abuse of cannabis. Compr Psychiatry (2012) 53: 1063–70. doi: 10. 1016/j. comppsych. 2012. 04. 013

||

31. Scott ES, Steinberg L. Adolescent development and the regulation of youth crime. Future Child.(2008) 18: 15–33. doi: 10. 1353/foc. 0. 0011

||

32. Rakhshan P, Pitts SC, Millman ZB, Andorko N, Wilson C, Demro C, et al. The impact of age on the validity of the prime screen in a sample of help-seeking adolescents. In: Poster Presentation, 11th International Conference on Early Intervention in Mental Health . Boston, MA (2018).

33. van Os J, Guloksuz S. A critique of the “ ultra high risk” and “ transition” paradigm. World Psychiatry (2017) 16: 200–6. doi: 10. 1002/wps. 20423

||

34. Mackie CJ, O’Leary-Barrett M, Al-Khudhairy N, Castellanos-Ryan N, Struve M, Topper L, et al. Adolescent bullying, cannabis use and emerging psychotic experiences: a longitudinal general population study. Psychol Med.(2013) 43: 1033–44. doi: 10. 1017/S003329171200205X

||

35. Vargas T, Osborn J, Cibelli E, Mittal VA. Separating hearing sensitivity from auditory perceptual abnormalities in Clinical High Risk (CHR) youth. Schizophr Res.(2018). doi: 10. 1016/j. schres. 2018. 08. 034. [Epub ahead of print].

||

36. Cowan R, McAdams D, Mittal VA. Core beliefs in late adolescence: factor structure and implications for psychosis, depression, and anxiety. Dev Psychopathol.(2018). doi: 10. 1017/S0954579417001912. [Epub ahead of print].

37. Robustelli B, Newberry R, Whisman M, Mittal VA. Social relationships and romantic relationships for youth at risk for psychosis: do they benefit? Psychiatry Res.(2017) 247: 345–51. doi: 10. 1016/j. psychres. 2016. 12. 008

|

38. Pelletier A, Ives L, Mittal VA. Increased internet use is associated with poorer ability to manage emotions in youth at high-risk for psychosis. Schizophr Res Cogn.(2015) 2: 220–6. doi: 10. 1016/j. scog. 2015. 08. 002

|

Thank's for Your Vote!
Individual differences and psychosis-risk screening: practical suggestions to improve the scope and quality of early identification. Page 1
Individual differences and psychosis-risk screening: practical suggestions to improve the scope and quality of early identification. Page 2
Individual differences and psychosis-risk screening: practical suggestions to improve the scope and quality of early identification. Page 3
Individual differences and psychosis-risk screening: practical suggestions to improve the scope and quality of early identification. Page 4
Individual differences and psychosis-risk screening: practical suggestions to improve the scope and quality of early identification. Page 5
Individual differences and psychosis-risk screening: practical suggestions to improve the scope and quality of early identification. Page 6
Individual differences and psychosis-risk screening: practical suggestions to improve the scope and quality of early identification. Page 7
Individual differences and psychosis-risk screening: practical suggestions to improve the scope and quality of early identification. Page 8
Individual differences and psychosis-risk screening: practical suggestions to improve the scope and quality of early identification. Page 9

This work, titled "Individual differences and psychosis-risk screening: practical suggestions to improve the scope and quality of early identification" was written and willingly shared by a fellow student. This sample can be utilized as a research and reference resource to aid in the writing of your own work. Any use of the work that does not include an appropriate citation is banned.

If you are the owner of this work and don’t want it to be published on AssignBuster, request its removal.

Request Removal
Cite this Essay

References

AssignBuster. (2021) 'Individual differences and psychosis-risk screening: practical suggestions to improve the scope and quality of early identification'. 24 December.

Reference

AssignBuster. (2021, December 24). Individual differences and psychosis-risk screening: practical suggestions to improve the scope and quality of early identification. Retrieved from https://assignbuster.com/individual-differences-and-psychosis-risk-screening-practical-suggestions-to-improve-the-scope-and-quality-of-early-identification/

References

AssignBuster. 2021. "Individual differences and psychosis-risk screening: practical suggestions to improve the scope and quality of early identification." December 24, 2021. https://assignbuster.com/individual-differences-and-psychosis-risk-screening-practical-suggestions-to-improve-the-scope-and-quality-of-early-identification/.

1. AssignBuster. "Individual differences and psychosis-risk screening: practical suggestions to improve the scope and quality of early identification." December 24, 2021. https://assignbuster.com/individual-differences-and-psychosis-risk-screening-practical-suggestions-to-improve-the-scope-and-quality-of-early-identification/.


Bibliography


AssignBuster. "Individual differences and psychosis-risk screening: practical suggestions to improve the scope and quality of early identification." December 24, 2021. https://assignbuster.com/individual-differences-and-psychosis-risk-screening-practical-suggestions-to-improve-the-scope-and-quality-of-early-identification/.

Work Cited

"Individual differences and psychosis-risk screening: practical suggestions to improve the scope and quality of early identification." AssignBuster, 24 Dec. 2021, assignbuster.com/individual-differences-and-psychosis-risk-screening-practical-suggestions-to-improve-the-scope-and-quality-of-early-identification/.

Get in Touch

Please, let us know if you have any ideas on improving Individual differences and psychosis-risk screening: practical suggestions to improve the scope and quality of early identification, or our service. We will be happy to hear what you think: [email protected]