1,651
28
Essay, 5 pages (1200 words)

Food sharing and empathic emotion regulation: an evolutionary perspective

A commentary on

by Hamburg, M. E., Finkenauer, C., and Schuengel, C. (2014). Front. Psychol. 5: 32. doi: 10. 3389/fpsyg. 2014. 00032

The social aspects of food sharing have been largely neglected, particularly in psychology. Mother-infant relations are a notable exception; one where the positive emotional consequences of feeding are rightfully seen as rewarding to mothers. What about food sharing in adults? While much has been written about food sharing as an important form of resource sharing (see Kaplan and Gurven, 2005 ), scant attention has been given to other consequences although growing evidence reveals other aspects that are more emotional-psychological in nature ( Rozin, 1996 ), including its appearance in courtship ( Alley et al., 2013 ). The potential use of food sharing for empathic emotion regulation (EER) proposed by Hamburg et al. (2014 ; hereafter HFS) gives us yet another reason to correct this and examine the social aspects of food sharing.

HFS largely ground their proposal in a developmental perspective, suggesting an origin in the positive emotions engendered by food provisioning early in life. Unfortunately it is quite difficult, at best, to connect adult emotional responses to an alleged infantile origin. The EER proposal is supported by various empirical findings, including positive psychopharmacological effects and emotional associations of certain foods as well as emotional eating ( Geliebter and Aversa, 2003 ). Nonetheless, their proposal may prove more powerful if it rested on a more solid foundation. Does adding an evolutionary perspective add credence to their proposal or does it pose a challenge?

An evolutionary perspective on resource sharing leads to the expectation that the provider should expect a favorable cost/benefit ratio in the long run. Something in return is expected that, given its value and likelihood, makes sharing worthwhile (i. e., adaptive rather than maladaptive). The benefits may be obvious in some cases, such as parent-offspring relations, especially if kin selection is considered. Far less obvious cases may occur where the benefits are embedded in reciprocal altruism ( Trivers, 1971 ) with a relatively long time scale such that the benefits may not appear for some time. Moreover, the benefits need not be in kind and may take the form of a vast array of “ paybacks” ranging from increases in tolerance to concrete goods or services to improved social status. In addition, the benefits may be potential and not actualized, as in the case of coalition building where the “ assets” of others (e. g., in mutual defense) may never be called upon. The point is that there are good reasons to expect food sharing to be done for, broadly speaking, self-interests, even in the case of relatives, mates, and other special individuals. Thus, the reward value of improved emotional state in another may be superfluous and play little or no role in the initiation of food sharing.

Improving the emotional state of another is, however, a benefit that may incur reciprocity. Just as coalitions for defense or for sharing food, for example, can be expected as long as the odds of receiving benefits in return are high enough given the cost of help, so too can positive contributions to affective state. HFS provide a plausible case for expecting positive contributions to affective states of recipients and offer the intriguing suggestion that some reward takes the form of a boost to the provider’s affective state, in addition to increasing closeness in the relationship. This view fits nicely with the more general view of emotions as special states shaped by natural selection to increase fitness by enhancing our ability to respond adaptively to opportunities and challenges ( Nesse, 1990 ; Levenson, 1999 ). A more specific consideration of empathy, however, may lead to the expectation that it serves an adaptive epistemological role as well as a social role. Specifically, empathy may be adaptive due to helpful constraint on predictions of the future actions of others in addition to a serving a social role as a motivator for prosocial behavior ( de Vignemont and Singer, 2006 ). This potential dual function makes it harder to argue for the power of EER to promote food transfer. That is, the primary function of empathy may be to enhance social cognition (i. e., an epistemic purpose) rather than to encourage resource sharing. As such, empathy may promote food sharing by improving recognition of the needs of others even if EER has no direct role in influencing behavior.

Even without reciprocity, EER benefits may motivate food sharing due to a general evolved tendency in our highly social species to help others as long as they too express this tendency. Evidence for this form of prosocial behavior, called “ strong reciprocity,” in humans is reviewed by Gintis (2000). With this in mind, the payback provided by EER, even if rather weak in light of ample other reasons for food sharing, may be critical if it is sufficient to push our inherently prosocial tendencies into action. Advocates note that if our species evolved toward strong reciprocity we must have become likely to punish cheaters and non-cooperators as well as to cooperate with others (e. g., Fehr et al., 2002 ), otherwise cheating and deceit should be widespread and would often render prosocial behavior maladaptive. Such tendencies for punishment should also largely eliminate false emotional expressiveness being used as a device to elicit food transfer.

In conclusion, an evolutionary perspective suggests that caution is needed when interpreting the underlying motivation for food transfers but also provides a solid phylogenetic basis for a limited influence of EER on food sharing.

References

Alley, T. R., Brubaker, L. W., and Fox, O. M. (2013). Courtship feeding in humans?: the effects of feeding versus providing food on perceived attraction and intimacy in dyads. Hum. Nat . 24, 430–443. doi: 10. 1007/s12110-013-9179-7

||

de Vignemont, F., and Singer, T. (2006). The empathic brain: how, when and why? Trends Cogn. Sci . 10, 435–441. doi: 10. 1016/j. tics. 2006. 08. 008

||

Fehr, E., Fischbacher, U., and Gächter, S. (2002). Strong reciprocity, human cooperation, and the enforcement of social norms. Hum. Nat . 13, 1–25. doi: 10. 1007/s12110-002-1012-7

Geliebter, A., and Aversa, A. (2003). Emotional eating in overweight, normal weight, and underweight individuals. Eating Behav . 3, 341–347. doi: 10. 1016/S1471-0153(02)00100-9

||

Gintis, H. (2000). Strong reciprocity and human sociality. J. Theor. Biol . 206, 169–179. doi: 10. 1006/jtbi. 2000. 2111

||

Hamburg, M. E., Finkenauer, C., and Schuengel, C. (2014). Food for love: the role of food offering in empathic emotion regulation. Front. Psych . 5: 32. doi: 10. 3389/fpsyg. 2014. 00032

||

Kaplan, H., and Gurven, M. (2005). “ The natural history of human food sharing and cooperation: a review and a new multi-individual approach to the negotiation of norms,” in Moral Sentiments and Material Interests: The Foundations of Cooperation in Economic Life , eds H. Gintis, S. Bowles, R. Boyd, and E. Fehr (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 75–113.

Levenson, R. W. (1999). The intrapersonal functions of emotion. Cogn. Emotion 13, 481–504. doi: 10. 1080/026999399379159

Nesse, R. M. (1990). Evolutionary explanations of emotions. Hum. Nat . 1, 261–289. doi: 10. 1007/BF02733986

||

Rozin, P. (1996). Towards a psychology of food and eating: from motivation to module to model to marker, morality, meaning, and metaphor. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci . 5, 18–24. doi: 10. 1111/1467-8721. ep10772690

Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Q. Rev. Biol . 46, 35–57. doi: 10. 1086/406755

Thank's for Your Vote!
Food sharing and empathic emotion regulation: an evolutionary perspective. Page 1
Food sharing and empathic emotion regulation: an evolutionary perspective. Page 2
Food sharing and empathic emotion regulation: an evolutionary perspective. Page 3
Food sharing and empathic emotion regulation: an evolutionary perspective. Page 4
Food sharing and empathic emotion regulation: an evolutionary perspective. Page 5
Food sharing and empathic emotion regulation: an evolutionary perspective. Page 6
Food sharing and empathic emotion regulation: an evolutionary perspective. Page 7

This work, titled "Food sharing and empathic emotion regulation: an evolutionary perspective" was written and willingly shared by a fellow student. This sample can be utilized as a research and reference resource to aid in the writing of your own work. Any use of the work that does not include an appropriate citation is banned.

If you are the owner of this work and don’t want it to be published on AssignBuster, request its removal.

Request Removal
Cite this Essay

References

AssignBuster. (2021) 'Food sharing and empathic emotion regulation: an evolutionary perspective'. 18 December.

Reference

AssignBuster. (2021, December 18). Food sharing and empathic emotion regulation: an evolutionary perspective. Retrieved from https://assignbuster.com/food-sharing-and-empathic-emotion-regulation-an-evolutionary-perspective/

References

AssignBuster. 2021. "Food sharing and empathic emotion regulation: an evolutionary perspective." December 18, 2021. https://assignbuster.com/food-sharing-and-empathic-emotion-regulation-an-evolutionary-perspective/.

1. AssignBuster. "Food sharing and empathic emotion regulation: an evolutionary perspective." December 18, 2021. https://assignbuster.com/food-sharing-and-empathic-emotion-regulation-an-evolutionary-perspective/.


Bibliography


AssignBuster. "Food sharing and empathic emotion regulation: an evolutionary perspective." December 18, 2021. https://assignbuster.com/food-sharing-and-empathic-emotion-regulation-an-evolutionary-perspective/.

Work Cited

"Food sharing and empathic emotion regulation: an evolutionary perspective." AssignBuster, 18 Dec. 2021, assignbuster.com/food-sharing-and-empathic-emotion-regulation-an-evolutionary-perspective/.

Get in Touch

Please, let us know if you have any ideas on improving Food sharing and empathic emotion regulation: an evolutionary perspective, or our service. We will be happy to hear what you think: [email protected]