1,944
14
Process Essay, 6 pages (1500 words)

Editorial: a matter of bottom-up or top-down processes: the role of attention in multisensory integration

Editorial on the Research Topic

Our everyday environments are multisensory and our brains handle this rich information in an extremely efficient way. Yet, attention’s role in the process of multisensory integration (MSI) is still the object of intense debate. Whilst some evidence supports that attention guides MSI via top-down selection of inputs, others suggest that bottom-up integration can occur pre-attentively capitalizing on temporal and spatial correlations. Understanding the role of attention in MSI is further complicated by the fact that attention itself refers to a variety of different selection mechanisms. Thus, the interplay between attention and MSI can take many forms and lead, as evident in the literature, to mixed findings and apparent contradictions (e. g., Driver, 1996 ; Talsma et al., 2010 ; Jack et al., 2013 ; Macaluso et al., 2016 ). This Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience Research Topic aims at helping clarify the nature of this interplay by posing a specific and narrow question. The reader will find a collection of 10 empirical papers plus an opinion and a review article which can broadly be classified into those addressing the contribution of bottom-up processing in MSI, and those exploring top-down modulations.

One framework for exploring the interplay between attention and MSI is to assume that attention leads to reweighting of sensory information ( Bresciani and Ernst, 2007 ). Focusing on bottom-up contributions, address such potential reweighting using the Maximum Likelihood Estimate model. The effect of attention on the weighting of sensory information could be disentangled by measuring observer’s audio-tactile spatial estimates, showing that bottom-up attention increases precision and alters sensory weighting in MSI. These findings corroborate selective attention’s role in adjusting the brain’s computations for achieving an integrated multisensory percept. Keeping the bottom-up perspective, address visual search in the barn owl. Similar to ventriloquism (e. g., Pick et al., 1969 ) and visual search in humans ( Onat et al., 2007 ), the owls’ visual search behavior was modulated by sound, demonstrating that audio-visual interactions guided visual attention. Visual search mechanisms might be similar among mammalian and non-mammalian species, owing to correlations between visual and auditory events in nature. An ultimate demonstration of the effect of bottom-up processes would consist in showing MSI for sub-threshold stimuli, in the absence of top-down biases. take a step in this direction showing that the visibility of a visual event under continuous flash suppression (CFS) increases when a sound is congruent (instead of incongruent). Albeit, as the authors argue, possible top-down processes may still exert an influence, the CFS framework provides a clear conceptualization of the question of bottom-up versus top-down processes. study explores both attentional cuing via bottom-up temporal entrainment and spatial cuing of attention in unisensory and cross-modal events. Both temporal and spatial attention-MSI interactions facilitated behavioral responses: attention produced a response advantage when deployed in a bottom-up temporal-cuing fashion and via top-down spatial attention manipulations. However, there was no measurable interaction between the bottom-up and top-down processes observed.

This research topic also includes two review/opinion papers with different views on bottom-up MSI (;). interpret expressions of early multisensory interaction as integration . Such that integration phenomena are irreducible, albeit top-down control processes can regulate their expression. Talsma et al. instead argue that cross-modal interactions that take place early, requiring no role of attention, do not result in integration . This controversy suggests that we may be missing crucial evidence, or are looking at extant evidence from incongruous angles. Defining what is meant by “ integration” would already be an important step in the right direction.

Despite the attempts to find core, bottom-up MSI interactions, top-down attentional components may also determine the outcome of MSI (e. g.,). Whether these influences are general, or confined to specific contexts, is still a matter of debate. This Research Topic includes five articles that have identified top-down influences employing various manipulations of multisensory congruency .

In an attempt to disentangle bottom-up versus top-down contributions manipulate attentional load and observer goals. Audio-visual binding in the bounce-stream paradigm was modulated by spatial cueing, suggesting that attention alters temporal binding of audio-visual signals in this task. Attention produced a response advantage when deployed in a bottom-up temporal-cuing approach and via the top-down spatial attention manipulation. However, similar to Jones’ conclusion (), there was no measurable interaction between bottom-up and top-down processes. Employing an audio-visual congruency manipulation with the attentional blink paradigm, could show that task-irrelevant sounds influence detection of, and awareness to, a visual target. Increased awareness of visual inputs was based not only on the congruency of current sensory evidence but also on prior knowledge, hinting that top-down expectations affect decisions regarding multisensory events and enhance integration. addresses whether task-irrelevant stimuli modulate cross-modal processing of semantically-congruent cues, by neutralizing low-level contributions. Consistent with the idea of extensive processing of cross-modal semantic relations, their fMRI results reveal that semantic-congruency engages fronto-parietal networks related to visuo-spatial control. Consequently, one could think of semantic congruency as providing a bias signal that exerts influence (yet not dominance) on the competitive interplay between bottom-up and top-down processes for the control of processing resources. Once one accepts that top-down influences are pervasive in MSI, the question of content-dependency arises. study explores to what extent content congruency will determine low levels of information processing in MSI and illustrates that audio-visual correspondence relations derived from human movements exert an important influence on auditory deviant detection and even on cross-modal synchrony perception.

The relation between attention and MSI further increases in complexity when manipulating stimulus-elicited emotions . Only a few studies have investigated multisensory emotion processing, despite the importance of both emotions and MSI to adaptive behavior. establish that attentional instructions and audio-visual congruency modulate sensory dominance in emotion processing. This study highlights how important it is to provide participants with detailed and clear instructions when characterizing MSI-attention interactions. Finally, find no direct relationship between MSI and attention. Their study also investigates MSI of emotional signals, yet in patients with depression. The patients rated faces as more fearful when displayed with happy sounds and appeared impaired in processing positive auditory information even when task-irrelevant. Neurocognitive tests revealed that those patients had impaired attention, which was not related to their emotion perception. Thus, impaired attention cannot directly explain deficits in multisensory (emotional) processing.

Conclusion and where Do We Go From Here

The work presented in this Research Topic demonstrates that the relation between MSI and attention is complex and unlikely to be answered by one single study. By bringing together these diverse works we observe that stimulus context effects, such as spatial/temporal co-location (e. g.,) or semantic (e. g.,) and emotional congruency (e. g.,), as well as the goal of the observer, such as changing task for similar stimuli (e. g.,;) tend to characterize whether MSI will be modulated by top-down attentional effects (e. g.,;;) or will seem to occur preattentively (e. g.,;;;;). It is fair to say that the interplay depends on many factors and, in some situations, involves no direct relation between attention and MSI (e. g.,). Clearer and universally agreed definitions would limit the same results being used for different perspectives on the debate of attention’s role in MSI. Future research using standardized instructions and experimental designs, e. g. CFS, controlling for either bottom-up or top-down influences (or both) across different contexts and observer goals would help get closer to a resolution of this ongoing debate.

Author Contributions

JH, SS, and RA co-editored the Research Topic and wrote the editorial.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Bresciani, J. P., and Ernst, M. O. (2007). Signal reliability modulates auditory–tactile integration for event counting. Neuroreport 18, 1157–1161. doi: 10. 1097/WNR. 0b013e3281ace0ca

||

Driver, J. (1996). Enhancement of selective listening by illusory mislocation of speech sounds due to lip-reading. Nature 381, 66–68. doi: 10. 1038/381066a0

||

Jack, B. N., O’Shea, R. P., Cottrell, D., and Ritter, W. (2013). Does the ventriloquist illusion assist selective listening? J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 39, 1496–1502. doi: 10. 1037/a0033594

||

Macaluso, E., Noppeney, U., Talsma, D., Vercillo, T., Hartcher-O’Brien, J., and Adam, R. (2016). The curious incident of attention in multisensory integration: bottom-up vs. top-down. Multisens. Res. 29, 557–583. doi: 10. 1163/22134808-00002528

|

Onat, S., Libertus, K., and König, P. (2007). Integrating audiovisual information for the control of overt attention. J. Vis. 7: 11. doi: 10. 1167/7. 10. 11

||

Pick, H. L. Jr., Warren, D. H., and Hay, J. C. (1969). Sensory conflict in judgments of spatial direction. Percept. Psychophys. 6, 203–205. doi: 10. 3758/BF03207017

|

Talsma, D., Senkowski, D., Soto-Faraco, S., and Woldorff, M. G. (2010). The multifaceted interplay between attention and multisensory integration. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 400–410. doi: 10. 1016/j. tics. 2010. 06. 008

||

Thank's for Your Vote!
Editorial: a matter of bottom-up or top-down processes: the role of attention in multisensory integration. Page 1
Editorial: a matter of bottom-up or top-down processes: the role of attention in multisensory integration. Page 2
Editorial: a matter of bottom-up or top-down processes: the role of attention in multisensory integration. Page 3
Editorial: a matter of bottom-up or top-down processes: the role of attention in multisensory integration. Page 4
Editorial: a matter of bottom-up or top-down processes: the role of attention in multisensory integration. Page 5
Editorial: a matter of bottom-up or top-down processes: the role of attention in multisensory integration. Page 6
Editorial: a matter of bottom-up or top-down processes: the role of attention in multisensory integration. Page 7
Editorial: a matter of bottom-up or top-down processes: the role of attention in multisensory integration. Page 8

This work, titled "Editorial: a matter of bottom-up or top-down processes: the role of attention in multisensory integration" was written and willingly shared by a fellow student. This sample can be utilized as a research and reference resource to aid in the writing of your own work. Any use of the work that does not include an appropriate citation is banned.

If you are the owner of this work and don’t want it to be published on AssignBuster, request its removal.

Request Removal
Cite this Process Essay

References

AssignBuster. (2022) 'Editorial: a matter of bottom-up or top-down processes: the role of attention in multisensory integration'. 16 January.

Reference

AssignBuster. (2022, January 16). Editorial: a matter of bottom-up or top-down processes: the role of attention in multisensory integration. Retrieved from https://assignbuster.com/editorial-a-matter-of-bottom-up-or-top-down-processes-the-role-of-attention-in-multisensory-integration/

References

AssignBuster. 2022. "Editorial: a matter of bottom-up or top-down processes: the role of attention in multisensory integration." January 16, 2022. https://assignbuster.com/editorial-a-matter-of-bottom-up-or-top-down-processes-the-role-of-attention-in-multisensory-integration/.

1. AssignBuster. "Editorial: a matter of bottom-up or top-down processes: the role of attention in multisensory integration." January 16, 2022. https://assignbuster.com/editorial-a-matter-of-bottom-up-or-top-down-processes-the-role-of-attention-in-multisensory-integration/.


Bibliography


AssignBuster. "Editorial: a matter of bottom-up or top-down processes: the role of attention in multisensory integration." January 16, 2022. https://assignbuster.com/editorial-a-matter-of-bottom-up-or-top-down-processes-the-role-of-attention-in-multisensory-integration/.

Work Cited

"Editorial: a matter of bottom-up or top-down processes: the role of attention in multisensory integration." AssignBuster, 16 Jan. 2022, assignbuster.com/editorial-a-matter-of-bottom-up-or-top-down-processes-the-role-of-attention-in-multisensory-integration/.

Get in Touch

Please, let us know if you have any ideas on improving Editorial: a matter of bottom-up or top-down processes: the role of attention in multisensory integration, or our service. We will be happy to hear what you think: [email protected]