1,122
15
Essay, 10 pages (2500 words)

Toward a better understanding of social learning, social deciding, and other-regarding preferences

What makes us do things like cooperate with others, perform altruistic behaviors, or be empathetic toward others? What neural circuits compute, and how hormones modulate, social behaviors? Our brains were evolved to function in complex social environments, demanding us to naturally tune our behaviors to social information ( Wilson, 2000 ). Social behaviors often define who we are and give rise to individual identity as well as group identity. The papers appearing in the E-Book on Neural basis of social learning, social deciding, and other-regarding preferences cover a large part of current debate in, and expand our knowledge of, social behaviors from the perspectives of neural networks and neuromodulators involved, as well as unique behavioral strategies engaged. This collection addresses outstanding questions in the neurobiology of human and non-human animal social behaviors in the form of original research articles, reviews, and perspective-type papers.

Lee and Harris sets the stage by reviewing useful ways to effectively combine the knowledge gained from the tradition of social psychology research and more recently emerged research in neuroeconomics, in order to better understand social behaviors and their neural correlates ( Lee and Harris, 2013 ). Taking a neuroethological viewpoint, Gariépy et al. review the similarities and differences in social learning across human and other species, and discuss the neural circuits involved in social learning ( Gariépy et al., 2014 ). Focusing on the importance of economical computations involved in social behaviors, Hillman discusses the fundamental role of cost-benefit calculations for competitive social interactions ( Hillman, 2013 ). Furthermore, emphasizing the significance of social context in shaping social behaviors, van den Bos et al., in an original research article, provide novel evidence that social identity is a strong driver of costly competitive behavior in humans engaged in a multi-player auction game ( Van den Bos et al., 2013 ). The authors further show that the basal levels of testosterone could predict this competition-driven behavior, endorsing the notion that neuromodulators are crucial for context-dependent social processing.

Conceptualizing complex social behaviors in a computational framework is a challenging task. In a hypothesis and theory article, Chang proposes a coordinate transformation framework, a scaffold borrowed from the tradition of sensorimotor research ( Andersen et al., 1993 ; Snyder, 2000 ), for characterizing how individual neurons encode social variables about self and others during social interactions ( Chang, 2013 ). Furthermore, Zak and Barraza, in their review paper, propose a mathematical model for describing the phenomenon of collective action in order to help explain how empathy and other cognitive variables modulate altruistic behaviors in humans ( Zak and Barraza, 2013 ).

Several papers in this issue argue for a specialized role of medial prefrontal/frontal cortical regions for guiding social behaviors. In an opinion piece, Lavin et al. propose that the human anterior cingulate cortex serves as a network hub in the brain for integrating the neural processes underlying social context processing, decision-making, and empathy ( Lavin et al., 2013 ). In a perspective article, Apps et al. discuss a socially-specialized role of the gyrus portion of the human midcingulate cortex in predicting and monitoring decision outcomes when interacting with others ( Apps et al., 2013 ). Furthermore, in a review paper, Isoda and Noritake deliberate why the dorsomedial frontal cortex is specialized for mediating theory of mind or mentalizing by discussing the key functions of this region in executive inhibition, self-other distinction, prediction under uncertainty, and perception of other’s intention ( Isoda and Noritake, 2013 ). A recent finding demonstrating the role of this brain region in shifting one’s strategy in accordance with the strategy of a competitive opponent ( Seo et al., 2014 ) strongly supports their prediction.

No one will argue that mother-infant interaction is one of the most critical social behaviors that influence behaviors that last one’s entire lifespan. In an original research article, Strathearn and Kim report that the hemodynamic activations in the amygdala in mothers are strongly modulated by infant identity (own-infant vs. unknown-infant) and valence displayed (happy vs. sad faces) from the images of infants, indicating that positive or negative values associated with infant face are processed in strikingly different ways depending on social context ( Strathearn and Kim, 2013 ). In another original research article, Ho et al. investigated dispositional empathy and stress sensitivity in mothers during a maternal decision-making scenario and found that different components of maternal dispositional empathy map onto distinct parts of the brain, spanning various subcortical and cortical regions ( Ho et al., 2014 ).

Facial expression serves as a key source of social information in human and non-human primates. In a perspective article, Gothard reviews how distributed networks of sensory, motor, affective, as well as motivational systems coordinate to generate facial expression, focusing particularly on the pathways between the amygdala and the midcingulate areas for transforming emotion-related signals into facial motor signals ( Gothard, 2014 ). It is well known that salient features in the environment capture one’s attention ( Itti and Koch, 2000 ). Social stimuli, such as faces and vocal calls, are particularly powerful at evoking orientation, probably reflecting their evolutionary importance for survival and reproduction. In an original research article, Dal Monte et al. show that exogenous oxytocin boosts social attention in rhesus macaques while viewing the faces of conspecifics ( Dal Monte et al., 2014 ). The authors report that exogenous oxytocin increases gaze fixations to the eyes relative to the mouth, suggesting the role of oxytocin in actively distributing attentional resources toward the eye region when viewing faces. In another original research article on social attention, Ebitz et al. used a visual distractor task in rhesus macaques using social and non-social images to show that greater pupil constriction is observed when viewing social images, suggesting that pupillary responses involved in attention take social relevance into account ( Ebitz et al., 2014 ). Using a novel scene-based search array task, Solyst and Buffalo report that rhesus macaques spend more time viewing conspecific images when the images contain socially salient features, such as those with direct gaze and redder sex skin ( Solyst and Buffalo, 2014 ). The authors further demonstrate that this preferential viewing was not driven by lower-level saliency attributes in these images. Collectively, these articles illustrate how the brain prioritizes social information.

Recently, there has been much interest in testing how neurons sensitive to primary reinforcement and action monitoring respond to socially-rewarding events and socially-relevant actions ( Izuma et al., 2008 ; Smith et al., 2010 ; Yoshida et al., 2011 , 2012 ; Azzi et al., 2012 ; Báez-Mendoza et al., 2013a ; Chang et al., 2013 ). In a review paper, Báez-Mendoza and Schultz discuss the role of the striatal neurons in selectively integrating rewards and actions across self and others, potentially mediating credit assignment between rewards and agency during social interactions ( Báez-Mendoza and Schultz, 2013b ). In an original research article, Kazama et al. show new causal evidence that neonatal lesion to the orbitofrontal cortex, implicated in value representation in rhesus macaques ( Padoa-Schioppa and Assad, 2006 ; Kennerley et al., 2011 ), impairs the ability later in life in adjusting behavioral responses to changing reward values ( Kazama et al., 2014 ).

The papers appearing in the E-Book collectively highlight new advances and emerging interests in the field of social neuroscience. The field is rapidly growing, providing many interesting insights into the neural underpinnings of social behavior. However, many challenges lie ahead. How does the brain know when to enhance social processing? What are some fundamental computational principles in the shared neural circuits across social and non-social behaviors? How do different parts of the brain or distinct subpopulations of neurons orchestrate social computation? These are just some of the interesting questions and challenges that remain before us.

Author Contributions

Steve W. C. Chang and Masaki Isoda wrote the current paper and served as the editors of this Research Topic E-Book.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by K99/R00-MH099093 (Steve W. C. Chang) and JSPS KAKENHI 24300125 (Masaki Isoda).

References

Andersen, R. A., Snyder, L. H., Li, C. S., and Stricanne, B. (1993). Coordinate transformations in the representation of spatial information. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol . 3, 171–176. doi: 10. 1016/0959-4388(93)90206-E

Apps, M. A. J., Lockwood, P. L., and Balsters, J. H. (2013). The role of the midcingulate cortex in monitoring others’ decisions. Front. Neurosci . 7: 251. doi: 10. 3389/fnins. 2013. 00251

Azzi, J. C. B., Sirigu, A., and Duhamel, J.-R. (2012). Modulation of value representation by social context in the primate orbitofrontal cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A . 109, 2126–2131. doi: 10. 1073/pnas. 1111715109

Báez-Mendoza, R., Harris, C. J., and Schultz, W. (2013a). Activity of striatal neurons reflects social action and own reward. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A . 110, 16634–16639. doi: 10. 1073/pnas. 1211342110

Báez-Mendoza, R., and Schultz, W. (2013b). The role of the striatum in social behavior. Front. Neurosci . 7: 233. doi: 10. 3389/fnins. 2013. 00233

Chang, S. W. C. (2013). Coordinate transformation approach to social interactions. Front. Neurosci . 7: 147. doi: 10. 3389/fnins. 2013. 00147

Chang, S. W. C., Gariépy, J.-F., and Platt, M. L. (2013). Neuronal reference frames for social decisions in primate frontal cortex. Nat. Neurosci . 16, 243–250. doi: 10. 1038/nn. 3287

Dal Monte, O., Noble, P. L., Costa, V. D., and Averbeck, B. B. (2014). Oxytocin enhances attention to the eye region in rhesus monkeys. Front. Neurosci . 8: 41. doi: 10. 3389/fnins. 2014. 00041

Ebitz, R. B., Pearson, J. M., and Platt, M. L. (2014). Pupil size and social vigilance in rhesus macaques. Front. Neurosci . 8: 100. doi: 10. 3389/fnins. 2014. 00100

Gariépy, J.-F., Watson, K. K., Du, E., Xie, D. L., Erb, J., Amasino, D., et al. (2014). Social learning in humans and other animals. Front. Neurosci . 8: 58. doi: 10. 3389/fnins. 2014. 00058

Gothard, K. M. (2014). The amygdalo-motor pathways and the control of facial expressions. Front. Neurosci . 8: 43. doi: 10. 3389/fnins. 2014. 00043

Hillman, K. L. (2013). Cost-benefit analysis: the first real rule of fight club? Front. Neurosci . 7: 248. doi: 10. 3389/fnins. 2013. 00248

Ho, S. S., Konrath, S., Brown, S., and Swain, J. E. (2014). Empathy and stress related neural responses in maternal decision making. Front. Neurosci . 8: 152. doi: 10. 3389/fnins. 2014. 00152

Isoda, M., and Noritake, A. (2013). What makes the dorsomedial frontal cortex active during reading the mental states of others? Front. Neurosci . 7: 232. doi: 10. 3389/fnins. 2013. 00232

Itti, L., and Koch, C. (2000). A saliency-based search mechanism for overt and covert shifts of visual attention. Vision Res . 40, 1489–1506. doi: 10. 1016/S0042-6989(99)00163-7

Izuma, K., Saito, D. N., and Sadato, N. (2008). Processing of social and monetary rewards in the human striatum. Neuron 58, 284–294. doi: 10. 1016/j. neuron. 2008. 03. 020

Kazama, A. M., Davis, M., and Bachevalier, J. (2014). Neonatal lesions of orbital frontal areas 11/13 in monkeys alter goal-directed behavior but spare fear conditioning and safety signal learning. Front. Neurosci . 8: 37. doi: 10. 3389/fnins. 2014. 00037

Kennerley, S. W., Behrens, T. E. J., and Wallis, J. D. (2011). Double dissociation of value computations in orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate neurons. Nat. Neurosci . 14, 1581–1589. doi: 10. 1038/nn. 2961

Lavin, C., Melis, C., Mikulan, E., Gelormini, C., Huepe, D., and Ibañez, A. (2013). The anterior cingulate cortex: an integrative hub for human socially-driven interactions. Front. Neurosci . 7: 64. doi: 10. 3389/fnins. 2013. 00064

Lee, V. K., and Harris, L. T. (2013). How social cognition can inform social decision making. Front. Neurosci . 7: 259. doi: 10. 3389/fnins. 2013. 00259

Padoa-Schioppa, C., and Assad, J. A. (2006). Neurons in the orbitofrontal cortex encode economic value. Nature 441, 223–226. doi: 10. 1038/nature04676

Seo, H., Cai, X., Donahue, C. H., and Lee, D. (2014). Neural correlates of strategic reasoning during competitive games. Science 346, 340–343. doi: 10. 1126/science. 1256254

Smith, D. V., Hayden, B. Y., Truong, T.-K., Song, A. W., Platt, M. L., and Huettel, S. A. (2010). distinct value signals in anterior and posterior ventromedial prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci . 30, 2490–2495. doi: 10. 1523/JNEUROSCI. 3319-09. 2010

Snyder, L. H. (2000). Coordinate transformations for eye and arm movements in the brain. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol . 10, 747–754. doi: 10. 1016/S0959-4388(00)00152-5

Solyst, J. A., and Buffalo, E. A. (2014). Social relevance drives viewing behavior independent of low-level salience in rhesus macaques. Front. Neurosci . 8: 354. doi: 10. 3389/fnins. 2014. 00354

Strathearn, L., and Kim, S. (2013). Mothers’ amygdala response to positive or negative infant affect is modulated by personal relevance. Front. Neurosci . 7: 176. doi: 10. 3389/fnins. 2013. 00176

Van den Bos, W., Golka, P. J. M., Effelsberg, D., and McClure, S. M. (2013). Pyrrhic victories: the need for social status drives costly competitive behavior. Front. Neurosci . 7: 189. doi: 10. 3389/fnins. 2013. 00189

Wilson, E. O. (2000). Sociobiology: The New Synthesis, Twenty-Fifth Anniversary Edition . Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press.

Yoshida, K., Saito, N., Iriki, A., and Isoda, M. (2011). Representation of others’ action by neurons in monkey medial frontal cortex. Curr. Biol . 21, 249–253. doi: 10. 1016/j. cub. 2011. 01. 004

Yoshida, K., Saito, N., Iriki, A., and Isoda, M. (2012). Social error monitoring in macaque frontal cortex. Nat. Neurosci . 15, 1307–1312. doi: 10. 1038/nn. 3180

Zak, P. J., and Barraza, J. A. (2013). The neurobiology of collective action. Front. Neurosci . 7: 211. doi: 10. 3389/fnins. 2013. 00211

Thank's for Your Vote!
Toward a better understanding of social learning, social deciding, and other-regarding preferences. Page 1
Toward a better understanding of social learning, social deciding, and other-regarding preferences. Page 2
Toward a better understanding of social learning, social deciding, and other-regarding preferences. Page 3
Toward a better understanding of social learning, social deciding, and other-regarding preferences. Page 4
Toward a better understanding of social learning, social deciding, and other-regarding preferences. Page 5
Toward a better understanding of social learning, social deciding, and other-regarding preferences. Page 6
Toward a better understanding of social learning, social deciding, and other-regarding preferences. Page 7
Toward a better understanding of social learning, social deciding, and other-regarding preferences. Page 8
Toward a better understanding of social learning, social deciding, and other-regarding preferences. Page 9

This work, titled "Toward a better understanding of social learning, social deciding, and other-regarding preferences" was written and willingly shared by a fellow student. This sample can be utilized as a research and reference resource to aid in the writing of your own work. Any use of the work that does not include an appropriate citation is banned.

If you are the owner of this work and don’t want it to be published on AssignBuster, request its removal.

Request Removal
Cite this Essay

References

AssignBuster. (2022) 'Toward a better understanding of social learning, social deciding, and other-regarding preferences'. 25 August.

Reference

AssignBuster. (2022, August 25). Toward a better understanding of social learning, social deciding, and other-regarding preferences. Retrieved from https://assignbuster.com/toward-a-better-understanding-of-social-learning-social-deciding-and-other-regarding-preferences/

References

AssignBuster. 2022. "Toward a better understanding of social learning, social deciding, and other-regarding preferences." August 25, 2022. https://assignbuster.com/toward-a-better-understanding-of-social-learning-social-deciding-and-other-regarding-preferences/.

1. AssignBuster. "Toward a better understanding of social learning, social deciding, and other-regarding preferences." August 25, 2022. https://assignbuster.com/toward-a-better-understanding-of-social-learning-social-deciding-and-other-regarding-preferences/.


Bibliography


AssignBuster. "Toward a better understanding of social learning, social deciding, and other-regarding preferences." August 25, 2022. https://assignbuster.com/toward-a-better-understanding-of-social-learning-social-deciding-and-other-regarding-preferences/.

Work Cited

"Toward a better understanding of social learning, social deciding, and other-regarding preferences." AssignBuster, 25 Aug. 2022, assignbuster.com/toward-a-better-understanding-of-social-learning-social-deciding-and-other-regarding-preferences/.

Get in Touch

Please, let us know if you have any ideas on improving Toward a better understanding of social learning, social deciding, and other-regarding preferences, or our service. We will be happy to hear what you think: [email protected]