1,785
21
Essay, 7 pages (1600 words)

Sleep paralysis, "the ghostly bedroom intruder” and out-of-body experiences: the role of mirror neurons

Rapid eye movement (REM) sleep—for good reasons—is referred to as paradoxical sleep : our blood pressure, heart rate, and breathing become elevated. And electroencephalography (EEG) recordings show a peculiar, lower voltage, and mixed frequency pattern ( La Berge et al., 1981 ; Horne, 2013 ). In fact, the firing pattern of most neurons during REM sleep resemble those of wakefulness—and in some cases neurons fire in even more intense bursts (e. g., in the pons, lateral geniculate nucleus, and occipital cortex), than when we’re awake ( Kandel et al., 2000 ). This is not all too surprising, as we have our most vivid and emotionally-charged dreams during REM sleep, often involving a complex story plot. In order for us not to act out these dreams—and potentially hurt ourselves—our brain has an ingenious solution: it leaves us temporarily paralyzed from head to toe. This paralysis (postural atonia) is triggered by the pons (including the pontine reticular formation) and ventromedial medulla that suppress skeletal muscle tone during REM sleep—via inhibition of motor neurons in the spinal cord; through neurotransmitters GABA and glycine ( Brooks and Peever, 2012 ; Jalal and Hinton, 2013 ).

Occasionally, perceptual activation occurs (we start to wake up mentally), while under the “ spell” of REM paralysis. The result is a curious condition called sleep paralysis (SP), where the person is left “ trapped”—unable to move or speak upon falling asleep or upon awakening ( Hobson, 1995 ; Jalal et al., 2014a ). Intriguingly during SP, the sensory system is clear, and ocular, and respiratory movements remain intact, culminating in a state of semi-consciousness coupled with bodily paralysis ( Jalal and Hinton, 2013 ). While once thought to only arise in the context of narcolepsy—a rare autoimmune sleep disorder affecting <1% of the population ( Jalal, 2016 )—we now know that 20% of the general population have SP episodes ( Sharpless and Barber, 2011 ; Jalal and Hinton, 2013 ).

During SP, the vivid—and sometimes terrifying—dreams of REM sleep (REM mentation) can spill over into emerging wakefulness ( Jalal and Hinton, 2015 ). Hypnogogic or hypnopompic hallucinations occur in all sensory modalities, and include out-of-body experiences (OBE), and sensing and seeing the presence of menacing intruders in one’s bedroom ( Jalal and Hinton, 2013 ; Jalal and Ramachandran, 2014 ; Jalal et al., 2014b , 2015 ).

We have proposed that a functional disturbance of the (right) parietal cortex may give rise to the common “ bedroom intruder” hallucination seen during SP ( Jalal and Ramachandran, 2014 ). As described, the absence of afferent sensory signals might cause this disturbance of “ body image”; implicating regions such as the right superior parietal lobule (SPL) and the temporoparietal junction (TPJ)—critical for the construction of a neural representation of the body. Essential to this hypothesis, is the hallucinated projection of a genetically hardwired body-map (homunculus) due to conflicting (efferent and afferent) neural conduction. This hypothesis is broadly consistent with the finding that disrupting the TPJ using focal electrical stimulation can induce the feeling of an illusory “ other” shadow-like person mimicking one’s body postures ( Arzy et al., 2006 ); and that hyperactivity in the temporoparietal cortex of schizophrenics can lead to the misattribution of their own actions to others ( Farrer et al., 2004 ).

We further evoke the mirror neuron system (MNS) as crucial in giving rise to this “ intruder” hallucination. Neurons in area V5 of the premotor cortex fire when you make volitional movements. Intriguingly, a subset of them (10%), fire even when you merely watch another person performing the action. These neurons—dubbed mirror neurons—allow higher centers to say in effect “ the same cells are firing as would fire if I were about to reach out for the peanut—so that’s what the other person is intending to do” ( Rizzolatti et al., 1996 , 2001 ). Circuits performing analogous computations may be involved in reading the higher order intentions that are required for constructing a theory of mind (ToM), but this is still a matter of some debate.

The MNS allows you to temporarily detach yourself from your body and “ see” the world from another person’s vantage point. In other primates, this requires the physical presence of another individual—whereas, in humans, it might be that the MNS is sufficiently well connected that it allows you a virtual point of view (i. e., imagine what you would be seeing if you were in the other person’s place). However, even though you temporarily see the world from another’s location—you don’t literally leave your body (i. e., you don’t have an out-of-body experience [OBE]). This is because the MNS has multiple outputs, which are powerfully modulated by two sources. First, sensory afferents from the body—and, second—prefrontal cortex. The triadic interaction between MNS, prefrontal cortex (anterior to V5), and sensory feedback results in a dual representation—a feeling that you are “ out there” looking at someone else’s actions—while at the same time being fully anchored, here, and now in your own body ( Ramachandran, 2012 ).

This interaction involves a convergence of inputs in the right SPL, and their target zones in V5. Not surprisingly, damage to the prefrontal cortex sometimes results in echopraxia—i. e., miming what somebody near is doing. Analogously, the massive deafferentation of sensory input during SP would lead to a similar disinhibition of the MNS and its propensity to project its body into another individual—if you are a chimp—or another virtual body, if you are a human. A disturbance of these interactions would lead to the more florid manifestations of an alien abductor, bedroom intruder, or mysterious other—seen so frequently during SP. In addition, we suggest that OBEs during SP, likewise result from the massive deafferentation that occurs during REM sleep paralysis. These ideas could be explored using neuroimaging, to examine the selective activation of brain regions associated with mirror neuron activity, when the individual is hallucinating an intruder or having an OBE during SP.

Author Contributions

BJ and VR came up with the intellectual content of the article, and wrote up the article.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The reviewer LP and handling Editor declared their shared affiliation, and the handling Editor states that the process nevertheless met the standards of a fair and objective review.

References

Arzy, S., Seeck, M., Ortigue, S., Spinelli, L., and Blanke, O. (2006). Induction of an illusory shadow person. Nature 443, 287–287. doi: 10. 1038/443287a

||

Brooks, P. L., and Peever, J. H. (2012). Identification of the transmitter and receptor mechanisms responsible for REM sleep paralysis. J. Neurosci. 32, 9785–9795. doi: 10. 1523/JNEUROSCI. 0482-12. 2012

||

Farrer, C., Franck, N., Frith, C. D., Decety, J., Georgieff, N., d’Amato, T., et al. (2004). Neural correlates of action attribution in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 131, 31–44. doi: 10. 1016/j. pscychresns. 2004. 02. 004

||

Hobson, J. A. (1995). Sleep . New York, NY: Scientific American Library.

Horne, J. (2013). Why REM sleep? Clues beyond the laboratory in a more challenging world. Biol. Psychol. 92, 152–168. doi: 10. 1016/j. biopsycho. 2012. 10. 010

||

Jalal, B. (2016). How to make the ghosts in my bedroom disappear? Focused-attention meditation combined with muscle relaxation (MR therapy)—a direct treatment intervention for sleep paralysis. Front. Psychol. 7: 28. doi: 10. 3389/fpsyg. 2016. 00028

||

Jalal, B., and Hinton, D. E. (2013). Rates and characteristics of sleep paralysis in the general population of Denmark and Egypt. Cult. Med. Psychiatry 37, 534–548. doi: 10. 1007/s11013-013-9327-x

||

Jalal, B., and Hinton, D. E. (2015). Sleep Paralysis among Egyptian college students: a. association with anxiety symptoms (PTSD, trait anxiety, pathological worry). J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 203, 871–875. doi: 10. 1097/NMD. 0000000000000382

||

Jalal, B., and Ramachandran, V. S. (2014). Sleep paralysis and “ the bedroom intruder”: the role of a. the right superior parietal, phantom pain and body image projection. Med. Hypotheses 83, 755–757. doi: 10. 1016/j. mehy. 2014. 10. 002

||

Jalal, B., Romanelli, A., and Hinton, D. E. (2015). Cultural explanations of sleep paralysis in a. Italy: the pandafeche attack and associated supernatural beliefs. Cult. Med. Psychiatry 39, 651–664. doi: 10. 1007/s11013-015-9442-y

||

Jalal, B., Simons-Rudolph, J., Jalal, B., and Hinton, D. E. (2014b). Explanations of a. sleep Paralysis among Egyptian college students and the general population in Egypt and Denmark. Transcult. Psychiatry 51, 158–175. doi: 10. 1177/1363461513503378

||

Jalal, B., Taylor, C. T., and Hinton, D. E. (2014a). A comparison of self-report and interview methods for assessing sleep paralysis: a pilot investigation in Denmark and the United States. J. Sleep Disord. 3, 9–13. doi: 10. 4172/2325-9639. 1000131

|

Kandel, E. R., Schwartz, J. H., and Jessell, T. M. (2000). Principles of Neural Science, 4th Edn . New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

La Berge, S. P., Nagel, L. E., Dement, W. C., and Zarcone, V. P. (1981). Lucid dreaming verified by volitional communication during REM sleep. Percept. Mot. Skills 52, 727–732. doi: 10. 2466/pms. 1981. 52. 3. 727

||

Ramachandran, V. S. (2012). The Tell-Tale Brain: Unlocking the Mystery of Human Nature . London: Windmill Books.

Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V., and Fogassi, L. (1996). Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Cogn. Brain Res. 3, 131–141. doi: 10. 1016/0926-6410(95)00038-0

||

Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., and Gallese, V. (2001). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 661–670. doi: 10. 1038/35090060

||

Sharpless, B. A., and Barber, J. P. (2011). Lifetime prevalence rates of sleep paralysis: a systematic review. Sleep Med. Rev. 15, 311–315. doi: 10. 1016/j. smrv. 2011. 01. 007

||

Thank's for Your Vote!
Sleep paralysis, "the ghostly bedroom intruder” and out-of-body experiences: the role of mirror neurons. Page 1
Sleep paralysis, "the ghostly bedroom intruder” and out-of-body experiences: the role of mirror neurons. Page 2
Sleep paralysis, "the ghostly bedroom intruder” and out-of-body experiences: the role of mirror neurons. Page 3
Sleep paralysis, "the ghostly bedroom intruder” and out-of-body experiences: the role of mirror neurons. Page 4
Sleep paralysis, "the ghostly bedroom intruder” and out-of-body experiences: the role of mirror neurons. Page 5
Sleep paralysis, "the ghostly bedroom intruder” and out-of-body experiences: the role of mirror neurons. Page 6
Sleep paralysis, "the ghostly bedroom intruder” and out-of-body experiences: the role of mirror neurons. Page 7
Sleep paralysis, "the ghostly bedroom intruder” and out-of-body experiences: the role of mirror neurons. Page 8
Sleep paralysis, "the ghostly bedroom intruder” and out-of-body experiences: the role of mirror neurons. Page 9

This work, titled "Sleep paralysis, “the ghostly bedroom intruder” and out-of-body experiences: the role of mirror neurons" was written and willingly shared by a fellow student. This sample can be utilized as a research and reference resource to aid in the writing of your own work. Any use of the work that does not include an appropriate citation is banned.

If you are the owner of this work and don’t want it to be published on AssignBuster, request its removal.

Request Removal
Cite this Essay

References

AssignBuster. (2022) 'Sleep paralysis, "the ghostly bedroom intruder” and out-of-body experiences: the role of mirror neurons'. 7 November.

Reference

AssignBuster. (2022, November 7). Sleep paralysis, "the ghostly bedroom intruder” and out-of-body experiences: the role of mirror neurons. Retrieved from https://assignbuster.com/sleep-paralysis-the-ghostly-bedroom-intruder-and-out-of-body-experiences-the-role-of-mirror-neurons/

References

AssignBuster. 2022. "Sleep paralysis, "the ghostly bedroom intruder” and out-of-body experiences: the role of mirror neurons." November 7, 2022. https://assignbuster.com/sleep-paralysis-the-ghostly-bedroom-intruder-and-out-of-body-experiences-the-role-of-mirror-neurons/.

1. AssignBuster. "Sleep paralysis, "the ghostly bedroom intruder” and out-of-body experiences: the role of mirror neurons." November 7, 2022. https://assignbuster.com/sleep-paralysis-the-ghostly-bedroom-intruder-and-out-of-body-experiences-the-role-of-mirror-neurons/.


Bibliography


AssignBuster. "Sleep paralysis, "the ghostly bedroom intruder” and out-of-body experiences: the role of mirror neurons." November 7, 2022. https://assignbuster.com/sleep-paralysis-the-ghostly-bedroom-intruder-and-out-of-body-experiences-the-role-of-mirror-neurons/.

Work Cited

"Sleep paralysis, "the ghostly bedroom intruder” and out-of-body experiences: the role of mirror neurons." AssignBuster, 7 Nov. 2022, assignbuster.com/sleep-paralysis-the-ghostly-bedroom-intruder-and-out-of-body-experiences-the-role-of-mirror-neurons/.

Get in Touch

Please, let us know if you have any ideas on improving Sleep paralysis, “the ghostly bedroom intruder” and out-of-body experiences: the role of mirror neurons, or our service. We will be happy to hear what you think: [email protected]