1,100
18
Essay, 16 pages (4000 words)

Editorial: avoidance: from basic science to psychopathology

The Editorial on the Research Topic

As a means of coping, avoidance encompasses thoughts and efforts toward prevention of future aversive experiences and events. Avoidance has been and remains controversial. Avoidance is accepted as a construct in many areas of research, but is roundly disdained in others. Why is such a critical feature of coping both acknowledged as such, but almost reluctantly studied?

For one, avoidance is often conflated with fear. Fear is an emotion. Threat conditions which engender fear also engender a host of physiological and behavioral responses ( Ledoux, 2013 ). In animals, exposure to aversive stimuli or cues associated with aversive stimuli induce freezing, fleeing, or aggressive displays depending on the context of exposure—all behavioral manifestations of threat (). Responses to threat are relatively simple, engendered and refined through a circumscribed neural circuitry ( Ledoux and Muller, 1997 ; Delgado et al., 2008 ). Fear and defensive responses to threat are readily and almost universally acquired. Those under threat ( Shors and Servatius, 1997 ), stress ( Servatius and Shors, 1994 ), and fearful () have a generalized facilitation of associative learning making threat and fear more pervasive. The engendering of fear and its expression is a highly researched concept; advancements in fear and the neurobiology subsuming fear is among the most notable and exhaustive neurobiological achievements in the past half century.

By comparison, avoidance is a fairly sophisticated construct. Avoidance is the situational evaluation of likelihoods, efficacy of responses, and costs. Avoidance is often weighed against alternatives; alternatives with differing or competing motivations ( Beck et al., 2011 ; Fernando et al., 2014 ;;). For many applications and circumstances fear and avoidance seem to be inseparable, so the terms become conflated. In the vernacular, fear is an immediate response to stressors and fear motivates avoidance. Therefore, in many circumstances those avoiding are expected to be experiencing fear. However, the empirical literature provides ample evidence that the processes are distinct ( Bolles, 1968 ; Seligman and Johnston, 1973 ;) and while the neurocircuitry, such as the lateral habenula ( Shumake et al., 2010 ; Ilango et al., 2013 ) and cerebellum ( Steinmetz et al., 1993 ) overlaps ( Freeman et al., 1996 , 1997 ; Bravo-Rivera et al., 2014 ;;;), their influences on these processes potentially do not. Further distinguishing fear and threat from avoidance, septal ( Thomas and Van Atta, 1972 ; Hedges et al., 1975 ) and hippocampal lesions () are known to facilitate avoidance acquisition, whereas these brain regions are critically involved in fear conditioning when intact ( Kim et al., 1993 ; Desmedt et al., 1998 ; Knight et al., 2004 ).

As a research topic, avoidance all but disappeared through the 1990’s, a phenomenon that has been noted in a number of recent reviews ( Dymond and Roche, 2009 ;). Reduction in the study of avoidance stemmed from theoretical and practical considerations. In humans, the rise of institutional review boards and the reluctance of institutions and investigators to study reactions to aversive, painful stimulation or uncomfortable situations stymied progress. Added to these concerns, there were growing controversies regarding the role of awareness and instructional sets in human associative learning. Explicit information stemming from the consent form and instructions complicated experimental designs and interpretations of acquisition. Now there are but a few laboratories across the world with a vested interest in studying avoidance acquisition and extinction in humans, the TOPIC highlights several ( Myers et al., 2013 ; Schlund et al., 2013 ; Sheynin et al., 2015 ;;). Otherwise, avoidance and coping are primarily studied through self-report survey instruments which document coping strategies ( Snell et al., 2011 ; Ayers et al., 2014 ).

In animals, the meteoric rise of electrophysiological and molecular techniques made reductionistic procedures ever more popular. This was in the face of Bolles formulation of species specific defense reactions (SSDRs) ( Bolles, 1970 ). A reading of Bolles strongly suggests that the most popular applications of avoidance learning in animals were reducible to reflexes. Avoidance that relied on SSDRs would be difficult to distinguish from fear responses or their modification and would be better studied in clearer procedures. Bolles did not negate avoidance learning, but argued that avoidance was obscured by SSDRs and arbitrary avoidances provided clear evidence of avoidance, which would be slowly and incrementally acquired. The Bolles position muddled already difficult discussions concerning reinforcement in avoidance acquisition ( Bersh, 2001 ; Dinsmoor, 2001 ; Hineline, 2001 ). The many criticisms of avoidance learning and its proper interpretation became more and more inaccessible to the average reader and more esoteric in argument. The zeitgeist is avoidance responses either a SSDR or require the suppression of SSDRs. SSDRs reflect fear and fear is more clearly examined in freezing ( Fanselow and Poulos, 2005 ) or by examining its exaggeration of acoustic startle responses ( Davis, 2006 ) under conditions in which control procedures are established to reveal associativity. Although arbitrary responses provide clear evidence of avoidance (; Bravo-Rivera et al., 2014 ;), these procedures became more and more unpopular. An increase in demand for throughput (self-contained, relatively short, and easily scored procedures) is at odds with the seemingly slow development of avoidance. In an unfortunate happenchance, “ passive avoidance” remains in the parlance of behavioral neuroscience, but the high-throughput tasks and protocols to study “ passive avoidance” are essentially assessing punishment.

Modern theorists of avoidance have moved away from response dynamics to cognitive processes driving response dynamics. Humans and mammals form expectancies. Avoidance expression reflects propositional knowledge but also the context in which knowledge is to be expressed ( Seligman and Johnston, 1973 ; Lovibond et al., 2008 , 2009 ; Dymond and Roche, 2009 ). Knowledge is subject to error and error correction (;). The difficulties encountered in learning arbitrary responses may not rest in how unnatural such responses are to humans and animals ( Dinsmoor, 2001 ), but in the pressures of time/distance ( Fanselow and Lester, 1988 ) and a cost/benefit analysis. There is a need for conceptual bridges between propositional knowledge central to expectancy models of avoidance and animal research in which processes are resolvable to response dynamics (response selectivity, strength of responding, and probability of responding;).

Recently, the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) in the United States embarked on research domain criteria (RDoC) to facilitate integration across levels of analysis and between diagnostic boundaries. The Negative Valence System encompasses acute responses to threat (fear) and inferred threat (anxiety), with escape/avoidance learning and expression emerging with sustained threat. In the NIMH working group discussion, ambivalence was expressed concerning whether sustained threat is distinct from acute threat, except for the time dimension. An undercurrent is that the sustained threat dimension, and by implication avoidance and escape, is not distinctive of acute conditions. The bounding conditions of avoidance are not only the duration of threat (acute/sustained), but the perceived intensity of threat, its perceived proximity, and the utility of responses or efforts. For perceived proximity of time, parametric manipulations of signal-shock intervals illustrate this point. Shuttling as the requisite response (a modified SSDR) is efficiently acquired with CS-US intervals of 10–20 s ( Black, 1963 ). In lever press (not an SSDR) avoidance, escape behaviors predominate when signal-shock intervals are less than 20-s ( Berger and Brush, 1975 ), with very few avoidance responses expressed after days of training (). However, knowledge about avoidance is acquired; avoidance is not expressed (). Using a crossover design those trained with a 10-s warning signal and exhibiting nominal avoidance rates displayed greater than 60% avoidance when switched to 60-s warning signal—nearly asymptotic performance of those trained initially with a 60-s warning signal. As to stressor intensity, shuttle box avoidance is efficiently acquired with foot shocks of moderately low intensity (0. 2–0. 5 mA) ( Levine, 1966 ) with decrements apparent with shock intensity greater than 1. 0 mA ( Moyer and Korn, 1964 ). In contrast, lever press avoidance is efficiently acquired with shock intensities of 1. 0–2. 0 mA ( Berger and Brush, 1975 ; Servatius et al., 2008 ;). These features illustrate that avoidance acquired with arbitrary responses differ in a number of parameters from those modifying reflexive responses or “ natural” responses, which are in turn distinct from fear responses. On the other hand, recent work also shows fear is more nuanced as fear contributes to sustained processes such as foraging ( Kim et al., 2014 ).

In subsequent position papers concerning RDoCs, fear and threat processes feature prominently, whereas avoidance and coping do not ( Cuthbert et al., 2003 ; Insel et al., 2010 ; Cuthbert, 2015 ). This is indeed unfortunate. An opportunity to intensify efforts in avoidance research is being missed. The mental health implications are extensive. Psychologically healthy coping strikes a balance between avoidance (responding in anticipation of aversive stimulation) and escape (responding in the presence of the stimulation) and competing motivations of approach (;). Deviant forms of avoidance are evident in autism ( Richer, 1976 ), anxiety ( Ly and Roelofs, 2009 ); ( Kashdan et al., 2014 ), phobias, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD; North et al., 2004 ; Kashdan et al., 2009 ), major depression ( Ottenbreit et al., 2014 ) and suicide ( Dixon et al., 1991 ). Over-expression of avoidance, as in anxiety disorders and PTSD, insulates one from aversive thoughts or experiences at the expense of self-limiting interpersonal and environmental interactions. Under-expression of avoidance, as in depression or suicidality, unduly exposes one to aversive thoughts and experiences that would be otherwise controllable, severely depleting resources and progressing down a demoralizing spiral.

Diathesis models of mental illness capture avoidance biases as dynamic interactions of vulnerabilities (genes, epigenetics, personality, and developmental phases) with risk factors (psychological stressors, physical injuries) ultimately expressed as psychopathology. For example, behaviorally inhibited temperament, withdrawal in the face of social and nonsocial challenges, is a vulnerability factor for anxiety disorders ( Moffitt et al., 2007 ). Humans expressing behavioral inhibition (BI) display enhanced avoidance expression (), and enhanced new motor learning (; Holloway et al., 2014 ), especially under degraded contingencies ( Holloway et al., 2014 ;). Facilitated avoidance acquisition (;;;) and new motor learning ( Ricart et al., 2011a , b ) are also apparent in Wistar-Kyoto rats, an animal model of BI temperament. Further, avoidance extinction is typically more difficult to obtain than extinction of fear. This is likely amplified by individual differences (;). Uncovering of neurobiological processes biasing avoidance expression and extinction has the promise of providing targets for individualized therapeutics and treatments for a number of psychopathological disorders.

Hence, there continues to be a need for an integration of human and animal research focused on coping and in particular avoidance coping. Model systems of avoidance that allow for bidirectional modifications of acquisition, expression, and extinction—protocols that allow for increased as well as decreased expression—are useful in translating basic science to psychopathology. By extension, RDoC constructs should be sensitive to individual differences, both accentuating and diminishing in the appearance of avoidance.

An open discussion of what features constitute fear, threat, anxiety, and avoidance would not only benefit basic science and psychopathology, but areas of research that are otherwise ignoring the infighting and are making substantial progress in improving health (e. g., fear-avoidance model of pain Vlaeyen et al., 1995 ; Crombez et al., 2012 ).

Approach Avoidance: Have no fear!

Author Contributions

The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and approved it for publication.

Funding

Supported by the Stress and Motivated Behavior Institute through funding from the Department of Defense, Armaments Research Development Engineering Center.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Ayers, C. R., Castriotta, N., Dozier, M. E., Espejo, E. P., and Porter, B. (2014). Behavioral and experiential avoidance in patients with hoarding disorder. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 45, 408–414. doi: 10. 1016/j. jbtep. 2014. 04. 005

||

Beck, K. D., Jiao, X., Ricart, T. M., Myers, C. E., Minor, T. R., Pang, K. C., et al. (2011). Vulnerability factors in anxiety: strain and sex differences in the use of signals associated with non-threat during the acquisition and extinction of active-avoidance behavior. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 35, 1659–1670. doi: 10. 1016/j. pnpbp. 2011. 05. 002

||

Berger, D. F., and Brush, F. R. (1975). Rapid acquisition of discrete-trial lever-press avoidance: effects of signal-shock interval. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 24, 227–239. doi: 10. 1901/jeab. 1975. 24-227

||

Bersh, P. J. (2001). The molarity of molecular theory and the molecularity of molar theory. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 75, 348–350; discussion: 367–378. doi: 10. 1901/jeab. 2001. 75-348

||

Black, A. H. (1963). The effects of CS-US interval on avoidance conditioning in the rat. Can. J. Psychol. 17, 174–182.

|

Bolles, R. C. (1968). Autonomic indices of fear: the collapse of an idea. Psychol. Rep. 23, 1249–1250. doi: 10. 2466/pr0. 1968. 23. 3f. 1249

||

Bolles, R. C. (1970). Species-specific defense reactions and avoidance learning. Psychol. Rev. 77, 32. doi: 10. 1037/h0028589

|

Bravo-Rivera, C., Roman-Ortiz, C., Brignoni-Perez, E., Sotres-Bayon, F., and Quirk, G. J. (2014). Neural structures mediating expression and extinction of platform-mediated avoidance. J. Neurosci. 34, 9736–9742. doi: 10. 1523/JNEUROSCI. 0191-14. 2014

||

Crombez, G., Eccleston, C., Van Damme, S., Vlaeyen, J. W., and Karoly, P. (2012). Fear-avoidance model of chronic pain: the next generation. Clin. J. Pain 28, 475–483. doi: 10. 1097/AJP. 0b013e3182385392

||

Cuthbert, B. N. (2015). Research Domain Criteria: toward future psychiatric nosologies. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 17, 89–97.

|

Cuthbert, B. N., Lang, P. J., Strauss, C., Drobes, D., Patrick, C. J., and Bradley, M. M. (2003). The psychophysiology of anxiety disorder: fear memory imagery. Psychophysiology 40, 407–422. doi: 10. 1111/1469-8986. 00043

||

Davis, M. (2006). Neural systems involved in fear and anxiety measured with fear-potentiated startle. Am. Psychol. 61, 741–756. doi: 10. 1037/0003-066X. 61. 8. 741

||

Delgado, M. R., Nearing, K. I., Ledoux, J. E., and Phelps, E. A. (2008). Neural circuitry underlying the regulation of conditioned fear and its relation to extinction. Neuron 59, 829–838. doi: 10. 1016/j. neuron. 2008. 06. 029

||

Desmedt, A., Garcia, R., and Jaffard, R. (1998). Differential modulation of changes in hippocampal-septal synaptic excitability by the amygdala as a function of either elemental or contextual fear conditioning in mice. J. Neurosci. 18, 480–487.

|

Dinsmoor, J. A. (2001). Stimuli inevitably generated by behavior that avoids electric shock are inherently reinforcing. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 75, 311. doi: 10. 1901/jeab. 2001. 75-311

||

Dixon, W. A., Heppner, P. P., and Anderson, W. P. (1991). Problem-solving appraisal, stress, hopelessness, and suicide ideation in a college population. J. Couns. Psychol. 38, 51. doi: 10. 1037/0022-0167. 38. 1. 51

|

Dymond, S., and Roche, B. (2009). A contemporary behavior analysis of anxiety and avoidance. Behav. Anal. 32, 7–27.

|

Fanselow, M. S., and Lester, L. S. (1988). “ A functional behavioristic approach to aversively motivated behavior: predatory imminence as a determinant of the topography of defensive behavior,” in Evolution and Learning , eds R. C. Bolles and M. D. Beecher (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum), 185–211.

Fanselow, M. S., and Poulos, A. M. (2005). The neuroscience of mammalian associative learning. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 56, 207–234. doi: 10. 1146/annurev. psych. 56. 091103. 070213

||

Fernando, A. B., Urcelay, G. P., Mar, A. C., Dickinson, A., and Robbins, T. W. (2014). Safety signals as instrumental reinforcers during free-operant avoidance. Learn. Mem. 21, 488–497. doi: 10. 1101/lm. 034603. 114

||

Freeman, J. H. Jr., Cuppernell, C., Flannery, K., and Gabriel, M. (1996). Context-specific multi-site cingulate cortical, limbic thalamic, and hippocampal neuronal activity during concurrent discriminative approach and avoidance training in rabbits. J. Neurosci. 16, 1538–1549.

|

Freeman, J. H. Jr., Weible, A., Rossi, J., and Gabriel, M. (1997). Lesions of the entorhinal cortex disrupt behavioral and neuronal responses to context change during extinction of discriminative avoidance behavior. Exp. Brain Res. 115, 445–457. doi: 10. 1007/PL00005714

||

Hedges, A. S., Van Atta, L., and Thomas, J. B. (1975). Septal lesions facilitate the shift from conditioned escape to conditioned avoidance behavior in the rat. Physiol. Behav. 14, 25–30. doi: 10. 1016/0031-9384(75)90136-5

||

Hineline, P. N. (2001). Beyond the molar-molecular distinction: we need multiscaled analyses. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 75, 342–347; discussion: 367–378. doi: 10. 1901/jeab. 2001. 75-342

||

Holloway, J. L., Allen, M. T., Myers, C. E., and Servatius, R. J. (2014). Behaviorally inhibited individuals demonstrate significantly enhanced conditioned response acquisition under non-optimal learning conditions. Behav. Brain Res. 261, 49–55. doi: 10. 1016/j. bbr. 2013. 10. 041

||

Ilango, A., Shumake, J., Wetzel, W., Scheich, H., and Ohl, F. W. (2013). Electrical stimulation of lateral habenula during learning: frequency-dependent effects on acquisition but not retrieval of a two-way active avoidance response. PLoS ONE 8: e65684. doi: 10. 1371/journal. pone. 0065684

||

Insel, T., Cuthbert, B., Garvey, M., Heinssen, R., Pine, D. S., Quinn, K., et al. (2010). Research domain criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry 167, 748–751. doi: 10. 1176/appi. ajp. 2010. 09091379

||

Kashdan, T. B., Goodman, F. R., Machell, K. A., Kleiman, E. M., Monfort, S. S., Ciarrochi, J., et al. (2014). A contextual approach to experiential avoidance and social anxiety: evidence from an experimental interaction and daily interactions of people with social anxiety disorder. Emotion 14, 769–781. doi: 10. 1037/a0035935

||

Kashdan, T. B., Morina, N., and Priebe, S. (2009). Post-traumatic stress disorder, social anxiety disorder, and depression in survivors of the Kosovo War: experiential avoidance as a contributor to distress and quality of life. J. Anxiety Disord. 23, 185–196. doi: 10. 1016/j. janxdis. 2008. 06. 006

||

Kim, E., Kim, E. J., Yeh, R., Shin, M., Bobman, J., Krasne, F. B., et al. (2014). Amygdaloid and non-amygdaloid fear both influence avoidance of risky foraging in hungry rats. Proc. Biol. Sci. 281: 20133357. doi: 10. 1098/rspb. 2013. 3357

||

Kim, J. J., Rison, R. A., and Fanselow, M. S. (1993). Effects of amygdala, hippocampus, and periaqueductal gray lesions on short- and long-term contextual fear. Behav. Neurosci. 107, 1093–1098. doi: 10. 1037/0735-7044. 107. 6. 1093

||

Knight, D. C., Cheng, D. T., Smith, C. N., Stein, E. A., and Helmstetter, F. J. (2004). Neural substrates mediating human delay and trace fear conditioning. J. Neurosci. 24, 218–228. doi: 10. 1523/JNEUROSCI. 0433-03. 2004

||

Ledoux, J. E. (2013). The slippery slope of fear. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 155–156. doi: 10. 1016/j. tics. 2013. 02. 004

||

Ledoux, J. E., and Muller, J. (1997). Emotional memory and psychopathology. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 352, 1719–1726. doi: 10. 1098/rstb. 1997. 0154

||

Levine, S. (1966). UCS intensity and avoidance learning. J. Exp. Psychol. 71, 163–164. doi: 10. 1037/h0022690

||

Lovibond, P. F., Mitchell, C. J., Minard, E., Brady, A., and Menzies, R. G. (2009). Safety behaviours preserve threat beliefs: protection from extinction of human fear conditioning by an avoidance response. Behav. Res. Ther. 47, 716–720. doi: 10. 1016/j. brat. 2009. 04. 013

||

Lovibond, P. F., Saunders, J. C., Weidemann, G., and Mitchell, C. J. (2008). Evidence for expectancy as a mediator of avoidance and anxiety in a laboratory model of human avoidance learning. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 61, 1199–1216. doi: 10. 1080/17470210701503229

||

Ly, V., and Roelofs, K. (2009). Social anxiety and cognitive expectancy of aversive outcome in avoidance conditioning. Behav. Res. Ther. 47, 840–847. doi: 10. 1016/j. brat. 2009. 06. 015

||

Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Harrington, H., Milne, B. J., Melchior, M., Goldberg, D., et al. (2007). Generalized anxiety disorder and depression: childhood risk factors in a birth cohort followed to age 32. Psychol. Med. 37, 441–452. doi: 10. 1017/S0033291706009640

||

Moyer, K. E., and Korn, J. H. (1964). Effect of ucs intensity on the acquisition and extinction of an avoidance response. J. Exp. Psychol. 67, 352–359. doi: 10. 1037/h0039933

||

Myers, C. E., Moustafa, A. A., Sheynin, J., Vanmeenen, K. M., Gilbertson, M. W., Orr, S. P., et al. (2013). Learning to obtain reward, but not avoid punishment, is affected by presence of PTSD symptoms in male veterans: empirical data and computational model. PLoS ONE 8: e72508. doi: 10. 1371/journal. pone. 0072508

||

North, C. S., Pfefferbaum, B., Tivis, L., Kawasaki, A., Reddy, C., and Spitznagel, E. L. (2004). The course of posttraumatic stress disorder in a follow-up study of survivors of the Oklahoma City bombing. Ann. Clin. Psychiatry 16, 209–215. doi: 10. 1080/10401230490522034

||

Ottenbreit, N. D., Dobson, K. S., and Quigley, L. (2014). An examination of avoidance in major depression in comparison to social anxiety disorder. Behav. Res. Ther. 56, 82–90. doi: 10. 1016/j. brat. 2014. 03. 005

||

Ricart, T. M., De Niear, M. A., Jiao, X., Pang, K. C., Beck, K. D., and Servatius, R. J. (2011a). Deficient proactive interference of eyeblink conditioning in Wistar-Kyoto rats. Behav. Brain Res. 216, 59–65. doi: 10. 1016/j. bbr. 2010. 07. 005

||

Ricart, T. M., Jiao, X., Pang, K. C., Beck, K. D., and Servatius, R. J. (2011b). Classical and instrumental conditioning of eyeblink responses in Wistar-Kyoto and Sprague-Dawley rats. Behav. Brain Res. 216, 414–418. doi: 10. 1016/j. bbr. 2010. 08. 029

||

Richer, J. (1976). The social-avoidance behaviour of autistic children. Anim. Behav. 24, 898–906. doi: 10. 1016/S0003-3472(76)80020-6

||

Schlund, M. W., Hudgins, C. D., Magee, S., and Dymond, S. (2013). Neuroimaging the temporal dynamics of human avoidance to sustained threat. Behav. Brain Res. 257, 148–155. doi: 10. 1016/j. bbr. 2013. 09. 042

||

Seligman, M. E. P., and Johnston, J. C. (1973). “ A cognitive theory of avoidance learning,” in Contemporary Approaches to Conditioning and Learning , eds F. J. McGuigan and D. B. Lumsden (Washington, DC: Winston and Sons), 69–110.

Servatius, R. J., Jiao, X., Beck, K. D., Pang, K. C., and Minor, T. R. (2008). Rapid avoidance acquisition in Wistar-Kyoto rats. Behav. Brain Res. 192, 191–197. doi: 10. 1016/j. bbr. 2008. 04. 006

||

Servatius, R. J., and Shors, T. J. (1994). Exposure to inescapable stress persistently facilitates associative and nonassociative learning in rats. Behav. Neurosci. 108, 1101–1106. doi: 10. 1037/0735-7044. 108. 6. 1101

||

Sheynin, J., Moustafa, A. A., Beck, K. D., Servatius, R. J., and Myers, C. E. (2015). Testing the role of reward and punishment sensitivity in avoidance behavior: a computational modeling approach. Behav. Brain Res. 283, 121–138. doi: 10. 1016/j. bbr. 2015. 01. 033

||

Shors, T. J., and Servatius, R. J. (1997). The contribution of stressor intensity, duration, and context to the stress-induced facilitation of associative learning. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 68, 92–96. doi: 10. 1006/nlme. 1997. 3763

||

Shumake, J., Ilango, A., Scheich, H., Wetzel, W., and Ohl, F. W. (2010). Differential neuromodulation of acquisition and retrieval of avoidance learning by the lateral habenula and ventral tegmental area. J. Neurosci. 30, 5876–5883. doi: 10. 1523/JNEUROSCI. 3604-09. 2010

||

Snell, D. L., Siegert, R. J., Hay-Smith, E. J., and Surgenor, L. J. (2011). Associations between illness perceptions, coping styles and outcome after mild traumatic brain injury: preliminary results from a cohort study. Brain Inj. 25, 1126–1138. doi: 10. 3109/02699052. 2011. 607786

||

Steinmetz, J. E., Logue, S. F., and Miller, D. P. (1993). Using signaled barpressing tasks to study the neural substrates of appetitive and aversive learning in rats: behavioral manipulations and cerebellar lesions. Behav. Neurosci. 107, 941–954. doi: 10. 1037/0735-7044. 107. 6. 941

||

Thomas, J. B., and Van Atta, L. (1972). Hyperirritability, lever-press avoidance, and septal lesions in the albino rat. Physiol. Behav. 8, 225–232. doi: 10. 1016/0031-9384(72)90365-4

||

Vlaeyen, J. W., Kole-Snijders, A. M., Boeren, R. G., and Van Eek, H. (1995). Fear of movement/(re)injury in chronic low back pain and its relation to behavioral performance. Pain 62, 363–372. doi: 10. 1016/0304-3959(94)00279-N

||

Thank's for Your Vote!
Editorial: avoidance: from basic science to psychopathology. Page 1
Editorial: avoidance: from basic science to psychopathology. Page 2
Editorial: avoidance: from basic science to psychopathology. Page 3
Editorial: avoidance: from basic science to psychopathology. Page 4
Editorial: avoidance: from basic science to psychopathology. Page 5
Editorial: avoidance: from basic science to psychopathology. Page 6
Editorial: avoidance: from basic science to psychopathology. Page 7
Editorial: avoidance: from basic science to psychopathology. Page 8
Editorial: avoidance: from basic science to psychopathology. Page 9

This work, titled "Editorial: avoidance: from basic science to psychopathology" was written and willingly shared by a fellow student. This sample can be utilized as a research and reference resource to aid in the writing of your own work. Any use of the work that does not include an appropriate citation is banned.

If you are the owner of this work and don’t want it to be published on AssignBuster, request its removal.

Request Removal
Cite this Essay

References

AssignBuster. (2022) 'Editorial: avoidance: from basic science to psychopathology'. 12 January.

Reference

AssignBuster. (2022, January 12). Editorial: avoidance: from basic science to psychopathology. Retrieved from https://assignbuster.com/editorial-avoidance-from-basic-science-to-psychopathology/

References

AssignBuster. 2022. "Editorial: avoidance: from basic science to psychopathology." January 12, 2022. https://assignbuster.com/editorial-avoidance-from-basic-science-to-psychopathology/.

1. AssignBuster. "Editorial: avoidance: from basic science to psychopathology." January 12, 2022. https://assignbuster.com/editorial-avoidance-from-basic-science-to-psychopathology/.


Bibliography


AssignBuster. "Editorial: avoidance: from basic science to psychopathology." January 12, 2022. https://assignbuster.com/editorial-avoidance-from-basic-science-to-psychopathology/.

Work Cited

"Editorial: avoidance: from basic science to psychopathology." AssignBuster, 12 Jan. 2022, assignbuster.com/editorial-avoidance-from-basic-science-to-psychopathology/.

Get in Touch

Please, let us know if you have any ideas on improving Editorial: avoidance: from basic science to psychopathology, or our service. We will be happy to hear what you think: [email protected]